2021
DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104879
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Contrast perception as a visual heuristic in the formulation of referential expressions

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
2
0

Year Published

2022
2022
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5

Relationship

2
3

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 6 publications
(5 citation statements)
references
References 64 publications
1
2
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Listeners are often quicker to identify a referent when they are provided with a (technically redundant) color word, despite the utterance being longer (Arts et al, 2011; Gwendolyn et al, 2021; Mangold & Pobel, 1988; Paraboni et al, 2007; Paraboni & van Deemter, 2014; Sonnenschein & Whitehurst, 1982; Rubio-Fernandez, 2021; Tourtouri et al, 2019). Consistent with the findings from language comprehension studies, speakers are more likely to use redundant color words as a function of their visual salience (Long et al, 2021; Rubio-Fernandez, 2021; Rubio-Fernandez et al, 2020).…”
Section: Validating Speaker–listener Alignmentsupporting
confidence: 81%
“…Listeners are often quicker to identify a referent when they are provided with a (technically redundant) color word, despite the utterance being longer (Arts et al, 2011; Gwendolyn et al, 2021; Mangold & Pobel, 1988; Paraboni et al, 2007; Paraboni & van Deemter, 2014; Sonnenschein & Whitehurst, 1982; Rubio-Fernandez, 2021; Tourtouri et al, 2019). Consistent with the findings from language comprehension studies, speakers are more likely to use redundant color words as a function of their visual salience (Long et al, 2021; Rubio-Fernandez, 2021; Rubio-Fernandez et al, 2020).…”
Section: Validating Speaker–listener Alignmentsupporting
confidence: 81%
“…While previous work in this direction manipulated artificial object arrays, either simple (e.g., Belke, 2001) or complex ones (e.g., Elsner et al, 2018), and sometimes even movie clips depicting motion events (Von Stutterheim, Andermann, Caroll, Flecken, & Schmiedtová, 2012), the current study measured eye movements using static, photo-realistic scenes. Our overall finding that increased realism in such scenes leads to substantially longer SOTs and arguably more complex scanning patterns, but not to fundamentally different search strategies, is relevant for models of reference production: it suggests that testing such models against empirical data gathered with simplified stimuli (e.g., Chen & Van Deemter, 2023;Gatt et al, 2017;Long et al, 2021) can be valid, as long as the nature of the scenes does not require a reduction of the set of distractors for contextual, semantic reasons. In general, our findings lend support for the notion that distractors shape the production of referring expressions, an idea that has been central to many computational models of reference production (since Dale & Reiter, 1995), and also has been argued to underpin models of human reference production (see, e.g., Van Gompel, van Deemter, Gatt, Snoeren, & Krahmer, 2019 for discussion).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 87%
“…This lack of realism may affect reference production. For example, on the object level, color atypicality reinforces the pop‐out effect: stored knowledge about typical object colors makes atypically colored objects salient in the visual scene (Becker et al., 2007; Sedivy, 2003), and atypical colors are, therefore, more often referred to than typical colors (Degen et al., 2020; Long, Moore, Mollica, & Rúbio‐Fernandez, 2021; Westerbeek, Koolen, & Maes, 2015). On the scene level, visual realism affects saliency as well, in the sense that low‐level saliency cues are less dominant in predicting fixation regions for realistic scenes such as photographs (Henderson, Brockmole, Castelhano, & Mack, 2007).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Recent cross-linguistic studies comparing the production and real-time comprehension of modified descriptions (e.g., “the red cup” vs. “the plastic cup”) have revealed that speakers use redundant (or noncontrastive) modification when it facilitates their listener’s visual search for a referent given their adjective position (i.e., whether it is prenominal or postnominal; Rubio-Fernandez, 2021b; Rubio-Fernandez & Jara-Ettinger, 2021; Rubio-Fernandez et al, 2021; for computational models and further empirical evidence, see Jara-Ettinger & Rubio-Fernandez, 2022). Extensive experience with referential communication may also result in the development of heuristics , as when perceptual contrast triggers the use of redundant modification (Ferreira, 2019; Long et al, 2021). Therefore, experimental studies and computational models suggest that our experience as listeners informs the choices we make as speakers in referential communication.…”
Section: Part 1: a New Theory Of The Relation Between Language And So...mentioning
confidence: 99%