1991
DOI: 10.1098/rspb.1991.0125
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Contrast adaptation and contrast masking in human vision

Abstract: After a preliminary study of visual evoked potentials (VEPS) to a test grating seen in the presence of masks at different orientations, psychophysical data are presented showing the effects of adaptation and of masking on thresholds for detecting the same test grating. The test is a vertical grating of spatial frequency 2 cycles per degree; adapting and masking gratings differ from the test either in orientation or in spatial frequency. The effects of adaptation and masking are explained by a single mechanism … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

4
41
0

Year Published

2001
2001
2016
2016

Publication Types

Select...
4
4
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 152 publications
(45 citation statements)
references
References 18 publications
(20 reference statements)
4
41
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In distinction, the masking here is an increasing function of mask contrast. 7 This is consistent with several other psychophysical studies of cross-orientation masking (e.g., Ross & Speed, 1991;Foley, 1994;Meese & Holmes, 2002Meese, 2004;Baker et al, 2007b; and implies that the pathways underlying cross-oriented, superimposed masking do not saturate, at least for contrasts up to 45%. For q 5 2 (a square rule for suppression), it is unlikely that this is achieved by a single inhibitory cell because visual neurons do not accelerate with the square of contrast over such a wide range of contrasts.…”
Section: The Excitatory and Suppressive Exponents (P And Q)supporting
confidence: 87%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…In distinction, the masking here is an increasing function of mask contrast. 7 This is consistent with several other psychophysical studies of cross-orientation masking (e.g., Ross & Speed, 1991;Foley, 1994;Meese & Holmes, 2002Meese, 2004;Baker et al, 2007b; and implies that the pathways underlying cross-oriented, superimposed masking do not saturate, at least for contrasts up to 45%. For q 5 2 (a square rule for suppression), it is unlikely that this is achieved by a single inhibitory cell because visual neurons do not accelerate with the square of contrast over such a wide range of contrasts.…”
Section: The Excitatory and Suppressive Exponents (P And Q)supporting
confidence: 87%
“…Therefore, it is unlikely that the mask produces masking by within-channel excitation on this model either. & Speed, 1991;Foley, 1994;Olzak & Thomas, 2003;Holmes & Meese, 2004;Meese & Hess, 2004;.…”
Section: The Slope Of the Psychometric Functionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The addition of a high-contrast overlay mask, oriented orthogonal to the test target, raises psychophysical thresholds, although generally not to the same degree as the mask (pedestal) of the same orientation as the test (Ross and Speed, 1991;Ross et al, 1993;Meier and Carandini, 2002). Foley (1994) proposed a model of contrast masking that brought psychophysical data for overlay masking into agreement with physiological observations (Chen and Foley, 2004).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…However, contrast masking in the HVS is a phenomenon that occurs in a frequency-orientation decomposed domain. For example, the masking effect is maximum when the orientation of the masker and the target are parallel and decreases when their orientations are perpendicular [24]. The contrast masking models that we saw in Section 2.2 can capture these effects since they normalize each filter output by pooling the responses at the same location of other filters tuned to different orientations.…”
Section: Relation To Hvs Based Modelsmentioning
confidence: 99%