2015
DOI: 10.1167/15.3.14
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Continuous psychophysics: Target-tracking to measure visual sensitivity

Abstract: We introduce a novel framework for estimating visual sensitivity using a continuous target-tracking task in concert with a dynamic internal model of human visual performance. Observers used a mouse cursor to track the center of a two-dimensional Gaussian luminance blob as it moved in a random walk in a field of dynamic additive Gaussian luminance noise. To estimate visual sensitivity, we fit a Kalman filter model to the human tracking data under the assumption that humans behave as Bayesian ideal observers. Su… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1

Citation Types

7
105
1

Year Published

2018
2018
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
2

Relationship

2
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 59 publications
(118 citation statements)
references
References 19 publications
(22 reference statements)
7
105
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Thus, it is of paramount importance to develop methods that enable the rapid collection of high-quality data, that require little or no training, and that can be used with nontraditional populations including children. We are working to adapt target-tracking methods for continuous psychophysics for this purpose (Bonnen, Burge, Yates, Pillow, & Cormack, 2015).…”
Section: Measuring the Reverse Pulfrich Effect In The Clinicmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Thus, it is of paramount importance to develop methods that enable the rapid collection of high-quality data, that require little or no training, and that can be used with nontraditional populations including children. We are working to adapt target-tracking methods for continuous psychophysics for this purpose (Bonnen, Burge, Yates, Pillow, & Cormack, 2015).…”
Section: Measuring the Reverse Pulfrich Effect In The Clinicmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In theory, this can be studied 105 by examining how knowledge of variability from sensory, motor, and task sources, influ-106 ences motor decision-making (Wolpert and Landy, 2012). The majority of studies support 107 the hypothesis that humans plan actions consistent with accurate knowledge of their sen-108 sorimotor uncertainty (e.g., Augustyn Bonnen et al, 2015), with some exceptions (e.g., Zhang et al, 2013). 110 However, the degree to which this knowledge is consciously available to the person is highly 111 debatable (Augustyn and Rosenbaum, 2005).…”
Section: Introduction 28mentioning
confidence: 96%
“…The recency effect548 In the sensorimotor feedback process, incoming error signals inform upcoming action plans 549 and quickly become irrelevant(Todorov, 2004;Bonnen et al, 2015). In contrast, the goal 550 of performance monitoring for sensorimotor confidence is to accumulate error signals across 551 time, much like the accumulation of sensory evidence for perceptual decisions with a fixed 552 viewing time.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Our response-tracking approach follows several recent studies that have used mouse tracking to probe the cognitive processes underlying various choice tasks (e.g., Bonnen, Burge, Yates, Pillow, & Cormack, 2015;Dale, Kehoe, & Spivey, 2007;Farmer, Cargill, Hindy, Dale, & Spivey, 2007;Hehman, Stolier, & Freeman, 2015;Rheem, Verma, & Becker 2018;Spivey, Grosjean, & Knoblich, 2005). In a choice task, a measure is typically made of some function of the choice outcome.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Analyses of these mouse trajectories and their temporal dynamics can therefore provide insights into these underlying processes (Hehman et al, 2015). Response tracking has been used to examine language processing (Spivey et al, 2005), target selection in visual search (Song & Nakayama, 2008), visual sensitivity (Bonnen et al, 2015), and cognitive load (Rheem et al, 2018).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%