2009
DOI: 10.3758/mc.38.1.67
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Contextual memory and skill transfer in category search

Abstract: Searching for information needed to complete a desired action is a common everyday experience. Examples-such as finding the telephone number of a local department store, searching for a book title on Amazon, or locating the required troubleshooting steps to fix a computer glitchare both familiar and numerous. The relevant information is generally available in a directory, in a manual, or on the Internet and needs to be searched for, often by category.Consider an individual using a relatively unfamiliar word-pr… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
3
0

Year Published

2014
2014
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
4
1

Relationship

0
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 7 publications
(3 citation statements)
references
References 26 publications
0
3
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Of course, prior knowledge facilitates learning under a variety of circumstances, regardless of age (Anderson, 1981; Bransford & Johnson, 1972; Glynn, Britton, & Muth, 1985; Kole & Healy, 2007; Kole, Healy, Fierman, & Bourne, 2010; Schustack & Anderson, 1979). However, there are situations in which older adults benefit more from applying their knowledge than do younger adults, sometimes eliminating age differences in memory (Craik & Jennings, 1992; Hess, 1990, 2005; Hess & Pullen, 1996; Laurence, 1967a, 1967b; Reyna & Mills, 2007; Woodruff-Pak & Hanson, 1995).…”
Section: How Prior Knowledge Affects Older Adults’ Episodic Memoriesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Of course, prior knowledge facilitates learning under a variety of circumstances, regardless of age (Anderson, 1981; Bransford & Johnson, 1972; Glynn, Britton, & Muth, 1985; Kole & Healy, 2007; Kole, Healy, Fierman, & Bourne, 2010; Schustack & Anderson, 1979). However, there are situations in which older adults benefit more from applying their knowledge than do younger adults, sometimes eliminating age differences in memory (Craik & Jennings, 1992; Hess, 1990, 2005; Hess & Pullen, 1996; Laurence, 1967a, 1967b; Reyna & Mills, 2007; Woodruff-Pak & Hanson, 1995).…”
Section: How Prior Knowledge Affects Older Adults’ Episodic Memoriesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…First, we confirmed that representation processes are involved in both cued-recall and recognition performance. A higher representation level would enable the constitution of more efficient semantic representational networks, providing support for the integration, learning, and rehearsal of new information (Bouazzaoui et al, 2013; Craik & Jennings, 1992; Kole & Healy, 2007; Kole, Healy, Fierman, & Bourne, 2010; Umanath & Marsh, 2014). Schematic support (Craik & Bosman, 1992) refers to the use of prior representation to improve episodic memory performance by driving the organisation of information and formation of associations.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Although it is hoped that learners are flexible and adaptive so they can generalize the knowledge and skills they acquire to novel situations, research has shown that instead learning in many cases is highly specific to the training context, with little or no transfer of training when the test and training situations differ. These differences may be relatively minor, such as changes to context (e.g., Bjork & Richardson-Klavehn, 1989;Eich, 1985;Godden & Baddeley, 1975;Kole, Healy, Fierman, & Bourne, 2010), changes to stimuli (e.g., Bourne, Healy, Pauli, Parker, & Birbaumer, 2005;Rickard, Healy, & Bourne, 1994), or changes to required responses (e.g., Healy, Wohldmann, Sutton, & Bourne, 2006;Yamaguchi & Proctor, 2009), or they may be relatively major, such as changes to task procedures (e.g., Healy, Wohldmann, Parker, & Bourne, 2005).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%