1999
DOI: 10.1038/23456
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Context generalization in Drosophila visual learning requires the mushroom bodies

Abstract: The world is permanently changing. Laboratory experiments on learning and memory normally minimize this feature of reality, keeping all conditions except the conditioned and unconditioned stimuli as constant as possible. In the real world, however, animals need to extract from the universe of sensory signals the actual predictors of salient events by separating them from non-predictive stimuli (context). In principle, this can be achieved if only those sensory inputs that resemble the reinforcer in their tempo… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

11
235
1
1

Year Published

2000
2000
2015
2015

Publication Types

Select...
7
2

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 237 publications
(254 citation statements)
references
References 25 publications
11
235
1
1
Order By: Relevance
“…For example, flies learn to avoid an odor that has been paired with electric shock (classical olfactory conditioning) or a spatial location where they were exposed to a noxious heat stimulus (operant place learning). When the task is made more complex by the addition of irrelevant sensory stimuli, flies can still learn the relevant predictive cue [Liu et al, 1999]. In an even more complex form of behavioral plasticity, when the sexual advances of a male fruit fly are rebuffed by a nonreceptive female, the male responds with a prolonged reduction in his courtship activity (courtship conditioning) [Tompkins et al, 1983;Mehren et al, 2004].…”
Section: Rationale For Studying Mr In Drosophilamentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, flies learn to avoid an odor that has been paired with electric shock (classical olfactory conditioning) or a spatial location where they were exposed to a noxious heat stimulus (operant place learning). When the task is made more complex by the addition of irrelevant sensory stimuli, flies can still learn the relevant predictive cue [Liu et al, 1999]. In an even more complex form of behavioral plasticity, when the sexual advances of a male fruit fly are rebuffed by a nonreceptive female, the male responds with a prolonged reduction in his courtship activity (courtship conditioning) [Tompkins et al, 1983;Mehren et al, 2004].…”
Section: Rationale For Studying Mr In Drosophilamentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In addition to playing a critical role in olfactory learning (Erber et al, 1980;de Belle and Heisenberg, 1994;Krashes et al, 2007), MBs are also implicated in mediating or modulating some complex adaptive behaviors including visual context generalization (Liu et al, 1999), visual place learning (Mizunami et al, 1998d), choice behavior (Tang and Guo, 2001), courtship conditioning (McBride et al, 1999), and sleep (Joiner et al, 2006;Pitman et al, 2006).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It is now clear that in olfactory classical conditioning (Tully and Quinn, 1985) the CS-US association mostly occurs within mushroom bodies (MBs) (Dubnau et al, 2001;McGuire et al, 2001). With the operant conditioning paradigm, the MBs were demonstrated to be dispensable for the visual associative learning (Wolf et al, 1998) but were necessary for some cognition-like functions (Liu et al, 1999;Tang and Guo, 2001). However, further cellular or molecular evidences related to the visual associative learning and related functions remain unknown.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In this paradigm, the fly was trained to discriminate the flight direction with reference to two kinds of visual patterns where one pattern type was associated with heat punishment by this paradigm, such as context generalization (Liu et al, 1999), visual selective attention (Wu et al, 2000), and choice behaviour facing competing alternatives (Tang and Guo, 2001).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%