2018
DOI: 10.1037/cep0000145
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Context-dependent control of attention capture: Evidence from proportion congruent effects.

Abstract: There are several independent demonstrations that attentional phenomena can be controlled in a context-dependent manner by cues associated with differing attentional control demands. The present set of experiments provide converging evidence that attention-capture phenomena can be modulated in a context-dependent fashion. We determined whether methods from the proportion congruent literature (listwide and item- and context-specific proportion congruent designs) that are known to modulate distractor interferenc… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1

Citation Types

6
15
0

Year Published

2019
2019
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
7
1
1

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 15 publications
(21 citation statements)
references
References 51 publications
6
15
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The difference between the performance in the target shift and in the intermixed task reveals that the benefits of automation are higher and more significant when the subject was presented the contextual-cue. These results are in line with literature on context-specific proportion congruent effects showing evidence for flexible transfer of context-specific control across different items (Bugg & Crump, 2012;Crump, Brosowsky, & Milliken, 2016;Crump & Logan, 2010;Crump & Milliken, 2009;Crump, Milliken, Leboe-Mcgowan, Leboe-Mcgowan, & Gao, 2018).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 92%
“…The difference between the performance in the target shift and in the intermixed task reveals that the benefits of automation are higher and more significant when the subject was presented the contextual-cue. These results are in line with literature on context-specific proportion congruent effects showing evidence for flexible transfer of context-specific control across different items (Bugg & Crump, 2012;Crump, Brosowsky, & Milliken, 2016;Crump & Logan, 2010;Crump & Milliken, 2009;Crump, Milliken, Leboe-Mcgowan, Leboe-Mcgowan, & Gao, 2018).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 92%
“…First, typical LSPC effects were seen with the Stroop task at the biased training locations. We note that this is also the first demonstration of location-specific control with "intact" Stroop stimuli (i.e., response relevant color information appearing in the word; other researchers employing the Stroop task have presented the word and color patch independently in space and time; Crump et al, 2006, Crump et al, 2008, Crump & Milliken, 2009Crump et al, 2018). Secondly, there was no evidence of the transfer of these effects to nearby novel locations as had been found in Fig.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 61%
“…Taken together, the studies reviewed above provide compelling evidence that specific contextual cues, be they stimulus locations, features or individual items (identities), as well as temporal episodes, can become associated with particular conflict control settings and trigger them in a cue-driven, bottom-up manner. Importantly, though, context-control learning is not limited to conflict-control in Stroop-type tasks, as equivalent effects have also been documented in tasks that probe other components of cognitive control or attention, including task-switching (e.g., Crump and Logan, 2010;Leboe et al, 2008), response inhibition (e.g., Verbruggen and Logan, 2008), dual-tasking (e.g., Fischer et al, 2014;Surrey et al, 2017), Simon task (e.g., Hübner and Mishra, 2016) and attention capture (e.g., Crump et al, 2018). For instance, switch costs -longer response times and lower accuracy when one has to switch rather than to repeat a task set -are thought to reflect control processes of reconfiguring a task-set (Rogers & Monsell, 1995) and/or overcoming interference from a previous set (Allport, Styles, & Hsieh, 1994).…”
Section: Behavioral Evidence For Context-control Learningmentioning
confidence: 93%