2002
DOI: 10.1037/0003-066x.57.10.749
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Constructing knowledge: The role of graphs and tables in hard and soft psychology.

Abstract: Because graphs provide a compact, rhetorically powerful way of representing research findings, recent theories of science have postulated their use as a distinguishing feature of science. Studies have shown that the use of graphs in journal articles correlates highly with the hardness of scientific fields, both across disciplines and across sub-fields of psychology. In contrast, the use of tables and inferential statistics in psychology is inversely related to subfield hardness, suggesting that the relationshi… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
111
1

Year Published

2005
2005
2014
2014

Publication Types

Select...
6
3

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 114 publications
(116 citation statements)
references
References 51 publications
3
111
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Why would psychologists consider journals that typically include papers that contain more graphs than tables as reporting 'harder' science (Smith et al, 2002), but also be more likely to cite articles that contain fewer graphs than tables, and more model diagrams? The graphism thesis contains an internal contradiction; it assumes that scientists frequently use graphs to convince their peers of the strength of their findings, and also are routinely duped by this same persuasion technique.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Why would psychologists consider journals that typically include papers that contain more graphs than tables as reporting 'harder' science (Smith et al, 2002), but also be more likely to cite articles that contain fewer graphs than tables, and more model diagrams? The graphism thesis contains an internal contradiction; it assumes that scientists frequently use graphs to convince their peers of the strength of their findings, and also are routinely duped by this same persuasion technique.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A well-designed graph can be a vivid, memorable, and easy-to-understand depiction of quantitative information (Larkin & Simon, 1987;Shah, Freedman, & Vekiri, 2005;Smith, Best, Stubbs, Archibald, & Roberson-Nay, 2002;Tufte, 2001). For those reasons, graphs are used extensively in textbooks, scientific journals, and the popular print media (Shah et al, 2005;Zacks, Levy, Tversky, & Schiano, 2002).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In addition, Smith et al (2000) asked the same respondents to rate the scientific 'hardness' of the subfields of 25 journals published by the American Psychological Association. Again, they found an almost perfect linear relationship and a correlation of .93 between the mean fractional graph areas and the 'hardness' attributed to the scientific disciplines (which ranged from behavioral neuroscience to educational psychology, see also Smith, Best, Stubbs, Archibald, & Roberson-Nay 2002). Thus, there was a strong linear and positive relationship between perceived 'hardness' and the use of graphs both between different scientific disciplines and between different subfields of one single discipline (psychology).…”
Section: Graphs Visualizing Empirical Data As Plausibility Cuesmentioning
confidence: 78%
“…Thus, there was a strong linear and positive relationship between perceived 'hardness' and the use of graphs both between different scientific disciplines and between different subfields of one single discipline (psychology). In contrast, the prevalence of the two other major devices to describe quantitative information or relationships, equations and tables, has been found to be either uncorrelated with hardness (Arsenault, Smith, & Beauchamp 2006), or even inversely related (Smith et al 2002).…”
Section: Graphs Visualizing Empirical Data As Plausibility Cuesmentioning
confidence: 99%