2011
DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01577.x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Consequences of “Minimal” Group Affiliations in Children

Abstract: Three experiments (total N = 140) tested the hypothesis that 5-year-old children’s membership in randomly assigned ‘minimal’ groups would be sufficient to induce intergroup bias. Children were randomly assigned to groups and engaged in tasks involving judgments of unfamiliar ingroup or outgroup children. Despite an absence of information regarding the relative status of groups or any competitive context, ingroup preferences were observed on explicit and implicit measures of attitude and resource allocation (Ex… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

58
730
12
5

Year Published

2013
2013
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
6
3

Relationship

2
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 550 publications
(805 citation statements)
references
References 69 publications
58
730
12
5
Order By: Relevance
“…Five-year-olds have more experience interacting with social groups than do 4-year-olds, and have been shown to be very sensitive to group membership more generally (Dunham, Baron, & Carey, 2011). Thus it was predicted that the older children would likely show a stronger difference between conditions than the younger children.…”
Section: Ostracism)and)affiliation)) ) ) 5)mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Five-year-olds have more experience interacting with social groups than do 4-year-olds, and have been shown to be very sensitive to group membership more generally (Dunham, Baron, & Carey, 2011). Thus it was predicted that the older children would likely show a stronger difference between conditions than the younger children.…”
Section: Ostracism)and)affiliation)) ) ) 5)mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, they tend to remember more positive information about ingroups and tend to interpret ambiguous intergroup interactions in ways that favor the ingroup (Dunham et al, 2011;Dunham & Emory, 2014). Even more profoundly, we argue that children can be considered active consumers of information who make choices regarding what they consume.…”
Section: Research Highlightsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We test this prediction with five-and six-year-old children, the age at which sensitivity to minimal groups begins to be robust (Dunham et al, 2011;Dunham & Emory, 2014;Spielman, 2000). More generally, because children have recently joined school and have increasing opportunity to choose the type of information they consume though storybooks and other media, this is a particularly important period to examine how their choices influence the development of intergroup attitudes.…”
Section: Research Highlightsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…But at the very least, it seems clear that children who provide answers that deviate from this norm are not necessarily incorrect and that a child who has a different gender experience or knowledge of a different experience (e.g., the sibling of a gender-nonconforming child) than most is not automatically disordered or confused in some way (e.g., Fast & Olson, under review). As way of analogy, most children favor members of their social groups, even arbitrarily assigned ones (e.g., Dunham, Baron, & Carey, 2011)-this response is statistically the most common response-yet we would not argue that a child who violates this norm-for example, a child who showed no group bias-is incorrect, deviant, or otherwise problematic.…”
Section: Gender and Sex-diverse Individuals As Targets Of Social Percmentioning
confidence: 99%