2014
DOI: 10.1007/s11097-014-9398-3
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Consciousness and choking in visually-guided actions

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
3
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5
1

Relationship

1
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 7 publications
(5 citation statements)
references
References 53 publications
0
3
0
Order By: Relevance
“…For example, to test changes in performance in goalball (where competitors attempt to roll a ball into their opponents’ goal), the interceptive ability of skilled team handball players could be tested as a representative task. A second potential limitation is that athletes who do not have VI have learned to play the sport with the benefit of vision, and it could be that some elements of their performance are so well-learned that their skills are more ‘automatic’ and rely on different sources of information to that of lesser-skilled athletes [ 40 , 41 ] or athletes with VI. Third, it is not possible to simulate an impairment to all the types of visual function that may be relevant to sport performance.…”
Section: Models For VI Classification Researchmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, to test changes in performance in goalball (where competitors attempt to roll a ball into their opponents’ goal), the interceptive ability of skilled team handball players could be tested as a representative task. A second potential limitation is that athletes who do not have VI have learned to play the sport with the benefit of vision, and it could be that some elements of their performance are so well-learned that their skills are more ‘automatic’ and rely on different sources of information to that of lesser-skilled athletes [ 40 , 41 ] or athletes with VI. Third, it is not possible to simulate an impairment to all the types of visual function that may be relevant to sport performance.…”
Section: Models For VI Classification Researchmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Unfortunately, despite the preceding findings, relatively little is known about the cognitive processes underlying QE. Thus, as Koedijker and Mann (2015) concluded, “the neural and functional mechanisms behind QE are largely unclear” (p. 336)—a view shared by Frank, Land, and Schack (2016) and confirmed in a recent review by Gonzalez et al (2015). Identification of the neural substrates of QE is challenging, however, because as Mann, Dicks, Cañal-Bruland, and van der Kamp (2013) argued, “it is difficult to make meaningful inferences about perceptual-motor expertise from experiments where participants cannot move” (p. 78).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A deliberate effort to train an action to become more ‘disguised’ presents an interesting challenge to the requirement for ‘intent’ in the definition of deception [ 28 ]. For example, Pete Sampras’s tennis serve was notoriously difficult to anticipate, reportedly because his junior coach during service drills would call out the required direction of serve only late in the service action [ 29 ].…”
Section: Deceptive Movements (Re-)definedmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Future research designs could assess both the deceptiveness (i.e. deceit and/or disguise) when acting as a performer and the perceptual ability when observing the same deceptive action (e.g., see [ 28 ]). Alternatively, observers could view their own deceptive actions (e.g.…”
Section: Mechanisms Underpinning the Perception Of Deceptive Actionsmentioning
confidence: 99%