2014
DOI: 10.1016/j.jml.2014.05.002
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Conjunction meaning can modulate parallelism facilitation: Eye-tracking evidence from German clausal coordination

Abstract: a b s t r a c tIn and-coordinated clauses, the second conjunct elicits faster reading times when it parallels (vs. does not parallel) the first in constituent order. This paper examined whether such parallelism facilitation results from simple constituent order priming from the first to the second clause, or whether it can be modulated through the linguistic context (the conjunction and clausal relations). Three eye-tracking experiments on German assessed this issue by manipulating conjunction meaning and type… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
24
1

Year Published

2017
2017
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
9

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 13 publications
(28 citation statements)
references
References 45 publications
3
24
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Second, it may be that after detecting the conjunct and, the comprehender generated a new prediction for upcoming structure, conditioned on how the first conjunct was produced. Previous work has argued for a processing-based parallelism preference on ATB configurations (Frazier et al 2000;Frazier & Clifton 2001;Apel et al 2007;Sturt et al 2010;Knoeferle 2014), that is, a preference that a gap in the subsequent conjuncts is located in the same relative position as it was in the first conjunct. This parallelism preference may be attributed to a predictive mechanism (Frazier et al 2000).…”
Section: What's Stored In Memory and How?mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Second, it may be that after detecting the conjunct and, the comprehender generated a new prediction for upcoming structure, conditioned on how the first conjunct was produced. Previous work has argued for a processing-based parallelism preference on ATB configurations (Frazier et al 2000;Frazier & Clifton 2001;Apel et al 2007;Sturt et al 2010;Knoeferle 2014), that is, a preference that a gap in the subsequent conjuncts is located in the same relative position as it was in the first conjunct. This parallelism preference may be attributed to a predictive mechanism (Frazier et al 2000).…”
Section: What's Stored In Memory and How?mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In such cases, it is clearly necessary for alternatives to exist (and to be sufficiently frequent that, in practice, participants can be induced to produce both alternatives). But there are other methods of inducing priming, such as priming of alternatives that differ in meaning (Scheepers 2003), priming choices in comprehension (e.g., Branigan et al 2005), priming of eye movements during comprehension (e.g., Arai et al 2007), priming response times in production (Smith & Wheeldon 2001;Segaert et al 2016), priming response times in comprehension (Knoeferle 2014), or repetition suppression of the Blood Oxygen Level Dependent (BOLD) signal in functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) adaptation (Segaert et al 2012). These methods do not always require semantically similar alternatives, and they should allow us to use priming more extensively in the future.…”
Section: R2 How Useful Is Priming?mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Our second study sidesteps the issue of parallelism by using but-instead of and-coordinated sentences. Unlike and, but evokes no expectation of parallelism between the two conjuncts, and indeed parallelism does not facilitate processing for but-conjoined sentences (Knoeferle 2014). The main goal of Experiment 2 was to investigate whether antecedent complexity effects in ellipsis processing are sensitive to task demands, as suggested by Phillips & Parker (2014) and Paape (2016).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%