2019
DOI: 10.1080/08838151.2019.1653101
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Conferring Resistance to Digital Disinformation: The Inoculating Influence of Procedural News Knowledge

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

9
55
0

Year Published

2019
2019
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
4
3
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 83 publications
(81 citation statements)
references
References 22 publications
9
55
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This suggests that, in cases where people actually do stop and think, relevant prior knowledge is likely to be a critical factor. Consistent with this suggestion, political knowledge was associated with an increased capacity to discern between true and false news content in an Italian sample [30], as were media literacy [57] and general information literacy [58]; and science knowledge in the context of (mis)information about COVID-19 [19,59].…”
Section: Box 2: Dual-process Models Of Reasoning and The Consequencesmentioning
confidence: 64%
“…This suggests that, in cases where people actually do stop and think, relevant prior knowledge is likely to be a critical factor. Consistent with this suggestion, political knowledge was associated with an increased capacity to discern between true and false news content in an Italian sample [30], as were media literacy [57] and general information literacy [58]; and science knowledge in the context of (mis)information about COVID-19 [19,59].…”
Section: Box 2: Dual-process Models Of Reasoning and The Consequencesmentioning
confidence: 64%
“…Previously shown to influence NA recognition (Amazeen & Wojdynski, 2018; Howe & Teufel, 2014), age was measured using an open-ended question ( M = 35.60, SD = 10.08). Knowledge about news practices/operations (procedural news knowledge) has been demonstrated to play a role in helping people identify and resist online covert advertising attempts (Amazeen & Bucy, 2019) and was based upon summing correct responses to three multiple choice questions about press releases, social media, and public media (see Supplemental Appendix C for question wording) that each had one correct answer. All three questions were correctly answered by 41% of the participants, one in three answered two correctly, 19% answered only one correctly, and 7% answered none of the questions correctly ( M = 2.08, SD = 0.93).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…After reverse-coding for consistent word polarity (with lower scores indicating less credibility), the five items were averaged to form a composite measure of perceived credibility of the political article (α = .90, M = 4.99, SD = 1.28). (Amazeen & Bucy, 2019) and was based upon summing correct responses to three multiple choice questions about press releases, social media, and public media (see online Appendix C for question wording) that each had one correct answer. All three questions were correctly answered by 41% of participants, one in three answered two correctly, 19% answered only one correctly, and 7% answered none of the questions correctly (M = 2.08, SD = 0.93).…”
Section: Credibility Of Contentmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Finally, recent attacks on the news media and use of the term “fake news,” which has garnered substantial public and academic attention (Amazeen & Bucy, 2019; Lazer et al, 2018), highlight the importance of understanding how audiences recognize news and its meaning. If news holds little meaning among the public, then calling something “fake news” would have little impact.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%