2010
DOI: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.05147.x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Conditioned taste aversion learning

Abstract: Drugs of abuse are typically discussed in terms of their rewarding effects and how these effects mediate drug taking. However, these drugs produce aversive effects that could have an important role in the overall acceptability of a drug and its likelihood of being self-administered. Rewarding and aversive effects, then, could be interpreted as separate behavioral effects, with the balance of the two determining overall drug acceptability. Interestingly, the role of aversive effects on drug acceptability in the… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
35
0

Year Published

2011
2011
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 70 publications
(37 citation statements)
references
References 210 publications
2
35
0
Order By: Relevance
“…These two inbred strains have been extensively used as animal vulnerability models for drug abuse. In brief, LEW animals (compared to the F344 rats) typically displayed greater acquisition of self-administration of several drugs of abuse, such as morphine and other opiates, nicotine, alcohol, and cocaine (for reviews, see Kosten and Ambrosio 2002;Davis and Riley 2010). The differences between these two strains in the SIP task also correlated with other indices of stress.…”
Section: Behavioral Genetics and Strain Differencesmentioning
confidence: 93%
“…These two inbred strains have been extensively used as animal vulnerability models for drug abuse. In brief, LEW animals (compared to the F344 rats) typically displayed greater acquisition of self-administration of several drugs of abuse, such as morphine and other opiates, nicotine, alcohol, and cocaine (for reviews, see Kosten and Ambrosio 2002;Davis and Riley 2010). The differences between these two strains in the SIP task also correlated with other indices of stress.…”
Section: Behavioral Genetics and Strain Differencesmentioning
confidence: 93%
“…Beginning in the late 1960s, it was discovered that drugs of abuse could suppress intake of an associated taste CS, the traditional behavioral hallmark of CTAs (e.g., Berger, 1972; Cappell & LeBlanc, 1971; Cappell, LeBlanc, & Endrenyi, 1973; Carey, 1973; Davison & House, 1975; Goudie, Dickins & Thornton, 1978; Kay, 1975; Le Magnen, 1969; Nachman, Lester, & Le Magnen, 1970; Nathan & Vogel, 1975; Riley, Jacobs, & LoLordo, 1978; Vogel & Nathan, 1975; for reviews see Davis & Riley, 2010; Hunt & Amit, 1987; Riley, 2011). Complicating the straightforward interpretation that drugs of abuse induce CTAs is the simple fact that the same drugs are self-administered by humans and other animals and support conditioned place preference learning (see Bardo & Bevins, 2000; Carr, Fibiger, & Phillips; 1989; Jaffe, 1970; Schechter & Calcagnetti, 1993; Schuster & Thompson, 1969; Tzschentke, 1998, 2007; van Rees, 1979; Weeks, 1962).…”
Section: Taste Reactivity and Palatabilitymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The nature of the US responsible for CTAs induced with drugs of abuse has been debated for many years (e.g., Davis & Riley, 2010; Gamzu, 1977; Goudie, 1979; Grigson et al, 2009; Hunt & Amit, 1987; Parker et al, 2009). One approach to this issue might involve the use pharmacological treatments, central or systemic, to investigate the influence of blocking specific properties of drug of abuse USs on CTA acquisition.…”
Section: Where Are We Now?mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Following repeated daily pairings, rats come to avoid intake of the saccharin cue in anticipation of drug availability relative to intake by saccharin-saline paired controls. This paradigm has, in fact, been studied for decades (Cappell & LeBlanc, 1971; Le Magnen, 1969), but was quickly interpreted as a conditioned taste aversion akin to that induced by the illness-inducing agent, LiCl (Davis & Riley, 2010; Lester, Nachman, & Le Magnen, 1970), and, as such, as evidence for aversive drug properties. We reinterpreted these data and hypothesized that rats avoid intake of the saccharin cue, at least in part, because the taste cue pales in perceived value relative to the potent drug of abuse expected in the very near future (Grigson, 1997).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%