2014
DOI: 10.1002/jeab.90
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Concurrent performance as bouts of behavior

Abstract: Log-survivor analyses of interresponse times suggest that the behavior of rats responding under single variable-interval schedules is organized into bouts (i.e., periods of engagement and disengagement). Attempts to generalize this analysis to the key pecking in pigeons, however, have failed to produce the characteristic broken-stick appearance typically obtained with rats. This failure may be due to a relatively low rate of reinforcement for engaging in alternative behavior experienced by pigeons. The present… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

2
39
0

Year Published

2014
2014
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 26 publications
(41 citation statements)
references
References 30 publications
(52 reference statements)
2
39
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Equation does not specify how bout lengths are distributed around their mean. Past models have suggested that bout lengths are geometrically distributed (Brackney et al, ; Shull et al, ), but have not provided empirical support (although see Smith et al, , Figures B, B, and 8B). The purpose of the present study was to characterize the distribution of bout lengths in free‐operant behavior.…”
mentioning
confidence: 96%
“…Equation does not specify how bout lengths are distributed around their mean. Past models have suggested that bout lengths are geometrically distributed (Brackney et al, ; Shull et al, ), but have not provided empirical support (although see Smith et al, , Figures B, B, and 8B). The purpose of the present study was to characterize the distribution of bout lengths in free‐operant behavior.…”
mentioning
confidence: 96%
“…NCR-induced bout-length effects may be partially explained by adventitious reinforcement of alternative behaviors that compete with the operant (Skinner, 1948). In such case, however, a decline in bout-initiation rate would also be expected (Smith et al, 2014). Alternatively, when reinforcement occurs while a rat is responding within a bout, the sudden reinforcer delivery may interrupt the bout and thus reinforce shorter bouts, with repeated interruptions gradually reducing the average bout length over time.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Within each bout, short inter-response times (IRTs) separate individual responses; longer IRTs separate bouts (Brackney, Cheung, Herbst, Hill, & Sanabria, 2012; Brackney, Cheung, Neisewander, & Sanabria, 2011; Brackney & Sanabria, 2015; Hill, Herbst, & Sanabria, 2012; Johnson, Pesek, & Newland, 2009; Shull, Gaynor, & Grimes, 2001, 2002; Shull, 2004, 2011; Smith, McLean, Shull, Hughes, & Pitts, 2014; Tolkamp, Schweitzer, & Kyriazakis, 2000). The response bout has three characteristics: (a) the mean bout length , or how many responses the animal makes while in a bout, (b) the within-bout response rate , or how fast the animal responds while in a bout, and (c) the bout-initiation rate , or how frequently the animal begins a new bout.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…They reported that responses (or IRTs) under single reinforcement schedules can be classified into bouts of responses or pauses, and that reinforcement frequency mainly affects the start of a bout (bout-initiation rate), whereas types of reinforcement schedule (VI and tandem VI-FR or tandem VI-VR) mainly affect the speed of responding within a bout (withinbout response rate). Because the reciprocal of the boutinitiation rate will be the time spent not responding, and that of the within-bout response rate will be the local response rate on the time spent in responding, Shull's results can be interpreted as showing that the reinforcement rate controls time allocation and IRT reinforcement controls local response rate (see also Smith, McLean, Shull, Hughes, & Pitts, 2014).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%