2007
DOI: 10.1080/15475440701360168
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Conceptual Foundations of Spatial Language: Evidence for a Goal Bias in Infants

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

13
117
1

Year Published

2008
2008
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
6
3

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 88 publications
(134 citation statements)
references
References 30 publications
13
117
1
Order By: Relevance
“…This results in striking errors, wherein children initially assume that ''milking the cow'' involves putting milk onto a cow! This bias likely stems from a more fundamental goal bias in language and thought, which has been documented extensively in previous work (e.g., Lakusta and Landau, 2005;Lakusta et al, 2007;Papafragou, 2010;Regier, 1997). This example suggests that cross-linguistic data from polysemy can provide evidence and perhaps new hypotheses about the nature of conceptual structure.…”
Section: Limitations Of Our Datamentioning
confidence: 56%
“…This results in striking errors, wherein children initially assume that ''milking the cow'' involves putting milk onto a cow! This bias likely stems from a more fundamental goal bias in language and thought, which has been documented extensively in previous work (e.g., Lakusta and Landau, 2005;Lakusta et al, 2007;Papafragou, 2010;Regier, 1997). This example suggests that cross-linguistic data from polysemy can provide evidence and perhaps new hypotheses about the nature of conceptual structure.…”
Section: Limitations Of Our Datamentioning
confidence: 56%
“…If precedence was the root cause of the current path bias, then the literature should be rife with process biases in all sorts of situations instead of the goal biases more commonly found. Moreover, there is a way to explicitly test this possibility with Directed Motion events, by using source objects instead of goal objects (cf Lakusta et al 2007). Instead of modeling an action path to a specific goal object, one could model a doll starting at a specific source object and then moving along an action path (stopping at the end of the trajectory but not at a specific object); thus, source precedes path.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…These data suggest that for grasping events, goal information trumps path information in infants' representations. Adopting this paradigm to Directed Motion events, Lakusta, Wagner, O'Hearn and Landau (2007) showed infants a toy animal moving from one of two source objects to one of two goal objects. At test, the animal either moved from a different source object or moved to a different goal object.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Thus all age groups have had relatively less experience with the passive than the active, and the results show that there are more congruent active responses than passive responses for all age groups. The active construction also aligns with a non-linguistic preference for conceptually framing events as source-to-goal or instigatorpatient rather than the reverse (Lakusta and Landau, 2005;Lakusta, Wagner, O'Hearn & Landau, 2007). Nappa et al (2009) note that in many languages, verbs that align with this preference (e.g., chase) outnumber their asymmetric partners (e.g., flee).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 64%