1967
DOI: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1967.tb01094.x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Components of Curiosity: Berlyne Reconsidered

Abstract: Berlyne has found that collative stimuli—stimuli with the attributes of complexity, incongruity, or ambiguity, among others—attract longer visual exploration than their less perplexing counterparts. He has postulated a curiosity motive to account for the finding; this paper suggests an alternative explanation of some of his results. The first of two experiments evaluates whether viewing time allotted to collative stimuli is increased by prior curiosity‐arousal. The second evaluates whether a ‘set to remember’ … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
7
0

Year Published

1974
1974
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
7
2

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 18 publications
(7 citation statements)
references
References 15 publications
0
7
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This notion has consistent empirical support, with a rich body of behavioral and neuroscientific evidence indicating that prediction errors releases dopamine and triggers new learning (e.g., the hypercorrection effect; Butterfield and Metcalfe 2006;Schultz et al 1997;Marvin and Shohamy 2016). This is because incongruity spurs greater cognitive engagement from making predictions, more active causal searches when expectations are not met, and conceptual change after a resolution is found (Brod et al 2018;Butterfield and Metcalfe 2006;Greenberger et al 1967;Itti and Baldi 2009). Unlike unpredictability, people inevitably engage in effortful causal searches once presented with incongruity, even when the resultant inferences are of little practical importance to them (Berlyne and Frommer 1966;Maheswaran and Chaiken 1991).…”
Section: The Power Of Surprisementioning
confidence: 70%
“…This notion has consistent empirical support, with a rich body of behavioral and neuroscientific evidence indicating that prediction errors releases dopamine and triggers new learning (e.g., the hypercorrection effect; Butterfield and Metcalfe 2006;Schultz et al 1997;Marvin and Shohamy 2016). This is because incongruity spurs greater cognitive engagement from making predictions, more active causal searches when expectations are not met, and conceptual change after a resolution is found (Brod et al 2018;Butterfield and Metcalfe 2006;Greenberger et al 1967;Itti and Baldi 2009). Unlike unpredictability, people inevitably engage in effortful causal searches once presented with incongruity, even when the resultant inferences are of little practical importance to them (Berlyne and Frommer 1966;Maheswaran and Chaiken 1991).…”
Section: The Power Of Surprisementioning
confidence: 70%
“…A novel experience is often recognized as a novel experience only when one is faced with situations in contrast to ones previous perceptions (Lee & Compton, 1992;Petrick, 2002). As a result, individuals are inclined to compare the present stimulation to previous experiences so as to evaluate the level of novelty (Greenberger, Woldman, & Yourshaw, 1967). Throughout the process of choosing travel destination, novelty-seeking is an important motivational factor.…”
Section: Novelty-seeking As a Segmentation Variablementioning
confidence: 98%
“…Others perceived this consumption trait as the degree of contrast between current stage and past experiences (Jenkins, 1969; Pearson, 1970). Since consumers find a product or situation novel when they have no previous experience, novelty seeking (or consumer innovativeness) is a relative trait that differs across individuals (Greenberger et al , 1967; Khare et al , 2010). Hirschman (1980) divided novelty seeking into two components: inherent and actualized.…”
Section: Literature Reviewmentioning
confidence: 99%