2016
DOI: 10.1016/j.bandc.2016.02.001
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Complimentary lower-level and higher-order systems underpin imitation learning

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1

Citation Types

2
2
0

Year Published

2018
2018
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
3

Relationship

3
0

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 3 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 52 publications
(9 reference statements)
2
2
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Nonetheless, both groups did scale hand and eye kinematics such that peak velocity occurred earlier in the movement trajectory when imitating the atypical compared to the typical model. As well as replicating previous findings in neurotypical participants ( Andrew et al, 2016 ; Hayes, Dutoy, et al, 2016 ), this is the first evidence showing that autistic individuals can imitate novel atypical biological kinematics that would not have existed in their motor repertoire.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 88%
“…Nonetheless, both groups did scale hand and eye kinematics such that peak velocity occurred earlier in the movement trajectory when imitating the atypical compared to the typical model. As well as replicating previous findings in neurotypical participants ( Andrew et al, 2016 ; Hayes, Dutoy, et al, 2016 ), this is the first evidence showing that autistic individuals can imitate novel atypical biological kinematics that would not have existed in their motor repertoire.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 88%
“…This modified experimental design would allow the direct examination of behavior change as a function of learning of a novel atypical stimulus via observational practice. In terms of our current results, the data for the neurotypical group is consistent with our previous finding (Andrew et al, 2016; Hayes et al, 2014; Hayes et al, 2016b) that neurotypical learners integrate visual information within a perception‐action matching mechanism (Prinz, 1997) containing visual‐motor integration processes (Bird et al, 2005; Cross et al, 2009; Higuchi et al, 2012; McGregor & Gribble, 2015). These processes underpin the representation of atypical biological kinematics as an action‐plan that controls subsequent movement reproduction.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 92%
“…Although the processes subserving imitation are operational across typical development (Anisfeld, 2005; Jones, 2009; Ray & Heyes, 2011), autistic individuals can exhibit processing differences that lead to specific motor imitation (Andrew et al, 2016; DeMyer et al, 1972; Hobson & Lee, 1999; Rogers et al, 1996; Stewart et al, 2013; Tunçgenç et al, 2021; Wild et al, 2012) difficulties when reproducing lower‐level biological kinematics displayed by a model (note, other forms of imitation such as automatic imitation are operational in autism; see de Hamilton et al, 2007; Vanvuchelen et al, 2013). These differences are likely to be underpinned by a network of interacting processes, such that autistic participants can sometimes show less perceptual sensitivity to the underlying biological motion properties (e.g., velocity) of an observed movement (Cook et al, 2009).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…To conclude, we have confirmed that atypical biological kinematics associated with an observed novel action are represented and reproduced following observational practice. Although we have previously shown this effect (Andrew, Bennett, Elliott, & Hayes, 2016; Hayes et al, 2014, 2016), the current data and Bayesian analyses extend theoretical knowledge of the processes underlying observational practice by implementing a methodology that controls movement direction of a model during action-observation and thus spatial compatibility. This method better isolates the representation of atypical kinematics to sensorimotor processes rather than spatial encoding.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 74%