2006
DOI: 10.1007/s10336-005-0037-1
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Competitive abilities of oystercatchers (Haematopus ostralegus) occupying territories of different quality

Abstract: In territorial species, habitat heterogeneity results in some individuals occupying poor quality sites while others occupy high quality sites. Floaters (mature nonbreeders) may accept a low quality territory, because it is the best they can get and defend ('inferior phenotype hypothesis'), or because it is a strategic alternative for a high quality territory in the long run ('queue hypothesis'). Oystercatcher Haematopus ostralegus territories differ consistently in the amount of offspring produced each year an… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
10
2

Year Published

2006
2006
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

2
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 11 publications
(13 citation statements)
references
References 43 publications
(45 reference statements)
1
10
2
Order By: Relevance
“…GPS data loggers allowed us to record even very distant foraging trips, as well as trips performed at night, neither of which could be recorded visually, given that the longest oystercatcher foraging trip was almost 6 km from the high tideline and thus out of range of tele- Although our data clearly support Ens et al's (1992) concept that leapfrog oystercatchers need to travel further to their foraging sites than residents, the concept of the birds having relatively static foraging territories on the intertidal mudflats ( Figure 1) is less certain, even though it has already been adopted in a series of follow-up studies (e.g., Hulsman, Zalucki, & Iedema, 1996;Heg et al, 2000;Bruinzeel et al, 2006; but see Kersten & Visser, 1996).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 45%
“…GPS data loggers allowed us to record even very distant foraging trips, as well as trips performed at night, neither of which could be recorded visually, given that the longest oystercatcher foraging trip was almost 6 km from the high tideline and thus out of range of tele- Although our data clearly support Ens et al's (1992) concept that leapfrog oystercatchers need to travel further to their foraging sites than residents, the concept of the birds having relatively static foraging territories on the intertidal mudflats ( Figure 1) is less certain, even though it has already been adopted in a series of follow-up studies (e.g., Hulsman, Zalucki, & Iedema, 1996;Heg et al, 2000;Bruinzeel et al, 2006; but see Kersten & Visser, 1996).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 45%
“…Many breeders were already banded when they were a nonbreeder, which allowed exact determination of the amount of breeding experience. In addition, we assumed that unbanded birds entering the breeding population had no previous breeding experience because many breeders in surrounding areas were also color banded and breeding dispersal is very local (van de Pol, Bruinzeel et al 2006). To verify that estimated breeding experience was an unbiased estimator of real breeding experience, we calibrated estimation rules on individuals that were ringed as juveniles.…”
Section: Pair-bond Duration Age and Breeding Experiencementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Floaters may also be individuals who reject all vacant breeding locations, preferentially waiting for access to a higher quality site that may increase their fitness (e.g. floaters queuing for a focal territory; Kokko & Sutherland, 1998;Pen & Weissing, 2000;Kokko, Harris & Wanless, 2004;Bruinzeel, van de Pol & Trierweiler, 2006;van de Pol et al, 2007).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%