2018
DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2017.09.005
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Competing perspectives on frames of reference in language and thought

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
2

Citation Types

0
10
0

Year Published

2018
2018
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
6
2
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 14 publications
(10 citation statements)
references
References 39 publications
0
10
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Finally, in a study comparing English‐ and Tzeltal‐speaking children's spatial reasoning, Li and Abarbanell (b; see also Abarbanell & Li, ) sought to revisit children's resistance to adopt language‐incongruent frames of reference reported in Haun et al. ().…”
Section: Spatial Frames Of Referencementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Finally, in a study comparing English‐ and Tzeltal‐speaking children's spatial reasoning, Li and Abarbanell (b; see also Abarbanell & Li, ) sought to revisit children's resistance to adopt language‐incongruent frames of reference reported in Haun et al. ().…”
Section: Spatial Frames Of Referencementioning
confidence: 99%
“…The present findings suggest that this question may be misguided. Rather than representing spatial relations egocentrically or allocentrically by default, people may use a variety of spatial FoRs flexibly in different contexts (and on different axes) starting early in life (64,(82)(83)(84)(85), according to the affordances of their spatial experience.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, most studies have relied on comparisons across human groups that differ not only in their spatial language but also in myriad other ways. To avoid the inferential complexities inherent to cross-cultural comparisons, some studies have evaluated within-group effects, comparing people's nonlinguistic FoR use (e.g., in spatial problem solving or co-speech gesture) with their mastery of egocentric spatial language (e.g., their ability to place objects on the "left" or "right"), with mixed results (12,22,23,28,29,(62)(63)(64)(65). However, if the goal is to clarify when and why people use a given FoR in a given context, then we must test not only people's knowledge of egocentric language but also their spontaneous use of it: Even people who can use egocentric spatial terms correctly in labeling tasks could disprefer those terms in everyday speech.…”
Section: Experiments 2: Frames Of Reference In Spatial Languagementioning
confidence: 99%
“…The present findings suggest that this question may be misguided. Rather than representing spatial relations egocentrically or allocentri-cally by default, people may use different spatial FoRs in different contexts (and on different axes) starting early in life (Acredolo & Evans, 1980;Li & Abarbanell, 2018), according to the perceptual and mnemonic demands of their spatial experience.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%