2011
DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.07.001
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Compatibility-sequence effects in the Simon task reflect episodic retrieval but not conflict adaptation: Evidence from LRP and N2

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

6
53
2

Year Published

2011
2011
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 55 publications
(61 citation statements)
references
References 35 publications
6
53
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Thus, on both occasions, the SP component seems to reflect control processes implemented to resolve interference due to a task conflict. In contrast, conflict adaptation in both the Simon and flanker tasks is typically associated with the N2 component, and this component is interpreted as interference due to a response conflict (Clayson & Larson, 2012;Spapé et al, 2011). Importantly, the N2 component was not present for the bivalency effect, which is consistent with previous findings that demonstrated that the bivalency effect occurs whether or not a response conflict is present .…”
supporting
confidence: 81%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Thus, on both occasions, the SP component seems to reflect control processes implemented to resolve interference due to a task conflict. In contrast, conflict adaptation in both the Simon and flanker tasks is typically associated with the N2 component, and this component is interpreted as interference due to a response conflict (Clayson & Larson, 2012;Spapé et al, 2011). Importantly, the N2 component was not present for the bivalency effect, which is consistent with previous findings that demonstrated that the bivalency effect occurs whether or not a response conflict is present .…”
supporting
confidence: 81%
“…In ERP research, three components that represent a signature of interference are typically discussed: the N2, the N450, and the sustained potential (SP). The N2 component represents a negative fronto-central deflection that emerges between 250 and 350 ms after stimulus onset (Clayson & Larson, 2011;Gehring, Gratton, Coles, & Donchin, 1992;Kopp, Rist, & Mattler, 1996;Spapé, Band, & Hommel, 2011). The N450 is also a frontocentral negative-going deflection that peaks approximately 450 ms after stimulus onset (Larson, Kaufman, & Perlstein, 2009;Liotti, Woldroff, Perez, & Mayberg, 2000;Markela-Lerenc et al, 2004;West, 2003;West, Bowry, & McConville, 2004;West, Jakubek, Wymbs, Perry, & Moore, 2005).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…These large switch effects on anterior N2 and parietal P3 are not specific to task switching paradigms, but are also evident in interference paradigms. For instance, N2 is larger for high versus low interference trials in Eriksen flanker and Simon tasks (Gehring, Gratton, Coles, & Donchin, 1992;Spape, Band, & Hommel, 2011), and P3 reduces with increasing attentional and working memory load (Donchin, Miller, & Farwell, 1986). Thus, the posttarget N2/P3b complex in task switching shows a pattern of modulation similar to that seen in classic interference paradigms (see Karayanidis & Jamadar, 2014, for review).…”
Section: Reactive Control In Task Switchingmentioning
confidence: 76%
“…It demonstrates the automatic impact of irrelevant spatial information on the induction of interfering action tendencies against people's will and has become a classic behavioral paradigm used to investigate cognitive control in laboratory studies (Hommel 2011;Spapé et al 2011). Simon studies have provided broad evidence that the Simon effect results from interference in the cognitive process (De Jong et al 1994;Leuthold 2011;Mansfield et al 2013;Martín-Arévalo et al 2015;Melara et al 2008;Wang and Weekes 2014;Wittfoth et al 2006).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%