1991
DOI: 10.1016/0167-6393(91)90039-v
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Comparison of prosodic properties between read and spontaneous speech material

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

2
38
0
4

Year Published

1992
1992
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
5
3

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 148 publications
(46 citation statements)
references
References 2 publications
2
38
0
4
Order By: Relevance
“…Second, there were more pauses in spontaneous style, which is consistent with previous observations (e.g., Howell and Kadi-Hanifi, 1991;Silverman, et al, 1992). The unplanned nature of spontaneous speech increases the number of pauses in this speech mode, many of which occur in unexpected locations such as inside syntactic phrases, for instance, inside a noun phrase between a noun and its modifier (tufan-ɛ [PAUSE] ʃaedid storm-EZ severe 'severe storm').…”
Section: Resultssupporting
confidence: 81%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Second, there were more pauses in spontaneous style, which is consistent with previous observations (e.g., Howell and Kadi-Hanifi, 1991;Silverman, et al, 1992). The unplanned nature of spontaneous speech increases the number of pauses in this speech mode, many of which occur in unexpected locations such as inside syntactic phrases, for instance, inside a noun phrase between a noun and its modifier (tufan-ɛ [PAUSE] ʃaedid storm-EZ severe 'severe storm').…”
Section: Resultssupporting
confidence: 81%
“…Spontaneous style also exhibits more phrase-final lengthening (White, et al, 2010;Markó and Kohári, 2015 for Hungarian) and faster speech rate (Laan, 1997;Furui, et al, 2005 for Japanese; Yeung, et al, 2008 for Mandarin;Dellwo, Leeman, and Kolly, 2015 for Swiss German). There are fewer, shorter, and more regular pauses and fewer disfluencies, e.g., repetitions or deletions, in read speech (Howell and Kadi-Hanifi, 1991;Silverman, et al, 1992;Megyesi and GustafsonČapková, 2002 for Swedish;Wang, Li, and Yuan, 2008 for Mandarin).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It is different from the prosody generated by other types of overt prosody such as spontaneous speech or disambiguating prosody. Spontaneous speech produces longer pauses but read speech produces more frequent tonal units or prosodic breaks, resulting in smaller prosodic phrases (Howell & Kadi-Hanifi, 1991;Silverman, Blaauw, Spitz, & Pitrelli, 1992;Ayers, 1994). In spontaneous speech or when speakers are trying to disambiguate an ambiguous structure, speakers generate prosody from the information structure they intend to deliver 1220 JUN (Price, Ostendorf, Shattuck-Hufnagel, & Fong, 1991;Allbritten, McKoon, & Ratcliff, 1996;Schafer, Speer, Warren, & White, 2000;Snedeker & Trueswell, 2003;Schafer, Speer, & Warren, 2005;Snedeker & Casserly, 2010 this issue).…”
Section: Implicit Prosody and Overt Prosody 1219mentioning
confidence: 98%
“…Jun (2010) has argued that it might not be possible to assess implicit prosody by comparing readers' overt read-aloud prosody to the judgments they make while or after silent reading. The prosody of read speech is easily recognizable as such, and differs substantially from spontaneous speech and "laboratory speech"(speech created for use in experiments), with shorter constituent phrases (so more pitch accents and breaks) (Howell & Kadi-Hanifi, 1991). In both laboratory speech and spontaneous speech, the speaker begins with an intended message-level meaning that contributes to the generation of a corresponding prosodic structure.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%