2020
DOI: 10.1002/jeab.623
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Comparison of delay discounting of different outcomes in cigarette smokers, smokeless tobacco users, e‐cigarette users, and non‐tobacco users

Abstract: Delay discounting is the process by which a commodity loses value as the delay to its receipt increases. Rapid discounting predicts various maladaptive behaviors including tobacco use. Typically, delay discounting of different outcomes has been compared between cigarette smokers and nonsmokers. To better understand the relationship of delay discounting to different modes of tobacco use, we examined the differences in delay discounting of different outcomes between cigarette smokers, smokeless tobacco users, e‐… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

1
9
0

Year Published

2021
2021
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
2

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 17 publications
(13 citation statements)
references
References 50 publications
1
9
0
Order By: Relevance
“…We administered a monetary delay discounting task in which participants were required to complete a total of 70 choices between an immediate reward and a delayed reward (see Figure 1A ). In accordance with previous studies ( Reynolds and Schiffbauer, 2004 ; Yi et al, 2017 ; DeHart et al, 2020 ), the delayed reward was always CNY100 presented at one of five delays (7, 15, 30, 60, 120 days). Delay periods were presented in blocks of 14 trials per delay.…”
Section: Methodssupporting
confidence: 62%
“…We administered a monetary delay discounting task in which participants were required to complete a total of 70 choices between an immediate reward and a delayed reward (see Figure 1A ). In accordance with previous studies ( Reynolds and Schiffbauer, 2004 ; Yi et al, 2017 ; DeHart et al, 2020 ), the delayed reward was always CNY100 presented at one of five delays (7, 15, 30, 60, 120 days). Delay periods were presented in blocks of 14 trials per delay.…”
Section: Methodssupporting
confidence: 62%
“…Behavioral outcomes are devalued as a function of the delay until they are experienced (for review, see Odum, 2011 ). This process, known as delay discounting, is reliably associated with cigarette smoking (for meta-analysis, see MacKillop et al, 2011 ; Amlung et al, 2016 ) and other tobacco use (e.g., Stein et al, 2018a ; DeHart et al, 2020 ). For example, high discounting rates for delayed monetary gains are cross-sectionally associated with smoking status (e.g., Mitchell, 1999 ) and longitudinally predict both smoking initiation ( Audrain-McGovern et al, 2009 ) and relapse following smoking cessation treatment (e.g., Yoon et al, 2007 ; Sheffer et al, 2014 ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Regarding substance use specifically, high rates of delay discounting relate to important aspects including: differentiating individuals dependent on substances from healthy controls (Businelle et al, 2010;Finn & Hall, 2004;Madden et al, 1997), substance use initiation (Audrain-McGovern et al, 2009), severity of use (Albein-Urios et al, 2012;MacKillop et al, 2010;Reynolds et al, 2004;Vuchinich & Simpson, 1998), and substance use treatment outcomes (Dallery & Raiff, 2007;Washio et al, 2011;Yoon et al, 2007). These findings are consistent across different substances of abuse, including alcohol (Amlung et al, 2017;MacKillop et al, 2010;Moody et al, 2017;Phung et al, 2019) and tobacco (Amlung et al, 2017;Barlow et al, 2017;Bickel et al, 2008;DeHart et al, 2020).…”
mentioning
confidence: 96%