2013
DOI: 10.1002/dev.21139
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Comparing human and nonhuman primate handedness: Challenges and a modest proposal for consensus

Abstract: In the past 20–25 years, there have been a number of studies published on handedness in nonhuman primates. The goal of these studies has been to evaluate whether monkeys and apes show patterns of hand preference that resemble the right-handedness found in the human species. The extant findings on handedness in nonhuman primates have revealed inconsistent evidence for population-level handedness within and between species. In this article, I discuss some of the methodological and statistical challenges to compa… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

2
56
0

Year Published

2015
2015
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 47 publications
(58 citation statements)
references
References 104 publications
2
56
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The determination of group-level hand preference is generally based on two factors: the strength of the individual hand preference (i.e., handedness index) and the number of individuals investigated [e.g., Papademetriou et al, 2005]. Because bonobos (and other non-human primates) rarely exclusively use one hand for particular tasks (i.e., they have a relatively low handedness index), larger sample sizes are considered necessary to reliably detect a group-level bias (defined as >65% of the individuals in the group) [Hopkins and Cantalupo, 2005;Hopkins et al, 2012;Hopkins 2013aHopkins , 2013b. In this study, the exclusive use of either the left-or right-hand (i.e., a high handedness index) by the 15 bonobo individuals suggests that use of the right-hand by 66% of the individuals may reliably estimate a group-level right-hand bias for this particular complex manipulative behaviour.…”
Section: Lateralitymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The determination of group-level hand preference is generally based on two factors: the strength of the individual hand preference (i.e., handedness index) and the number of individuals investigated [e.g., Papademetriou et al, 2005]. Because bonobos (and other non-human primates) rarely exclusively use one hand for particular tasks (i.e., they have a relatively low handedness index), larger sample sizes are considered necessary to reliably detect a group-level bias (defined as >65% of the individuals in the group) [Hopkins and Cantalupo, 2005;Hopkins et al, 2012;Hopkins 2013aHopkins , 2013b. In this study, the exclusive use of either the left-or right-hand (i.e., a high handedness index) by the 15 bonobo individuals suggests that use of the right-hand by 66% of the individuals may reliably estimate a group-level right-hand bias for this particular complex manipulative behaviour.…”
Section: Lateralitymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…See also Figure S2. and is not consistently expressed across a number of different behaviors ( [22][23][24], but see [25] for one exception). Consistent one-hand preference at the population level across a wide range of behaviors (so-called ''true'' handedness [26]) is considered to be a characteristic of humans [8,27,28].…”
Section: Figure 2 Lateralization Of Forelimb Use In Marsupialsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…not as pronounced as in humans [13][14][15]. Additionally, neuroimaging studies of chimpanzees have shown functional lateralization in Broca's area homologue related to communicative behaviour [16] and in the hand knob, the motor-hand region of the precentral gyrus, in relation to reach-and-grasping responses [17].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 97%