1998
DOI: 10.1515/ling.1998.36.4.807
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Comparing different models of the development of the English verb category

Abstract: In this study data from the first six months of 12 children's multiword speech were used to test the validity of Valians (1991) syntactic performance-limitation account andTomasello's (1992)

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

9
88
0
3

Year Published

2003
2003
2015
2015

Publication Types

Select...
4
4
1

Relationship

2
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 155 publications
(103 citation statements)
references
References 30 publications
9
88
0
3
Order By: Relevance
“…MOSAIC is therefore often regarded as a usage-based model of language learning (cf. Goldberg, 1995;Lieven, Pine & Baldwin, 1997;Pine, Lieven & Rowland, 1998;Tomasello, 2000;, MacWhinney, 2004. However, although the mechanisms implemented in MOSAIC are certainly consistent with a usage-based analysis, it is important to recognise that MOSAIC is a relatively simple distributional analyser, with no access to semantic information, which is not sufficiently powerful to acquire many aspects of adult syntax.…”
Section: An Alternative Account Of Optional Infinitive Errorsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…MOSAIC is therefore often regarded as a usage-based model of language learning (cf. Goldberg, 1995;Lieven, Pine & Baldwin, 1997;Pine, Lieven & Rowland, 1998;Tomasello, 2000;, MacWhinney, 2004. However, although the mechanisms implemented in MOSAIC are certainly consistent with a usage-based analysis, it is important to recognise that MOSAIC is a relatively simple distributional analyser, with no access to semantic information, which is not sufficiently powerful to acquire many aspects of adult syntax.…”
Section: An Alternative Account Of Optional Infinitive Errorsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Lieven, 2008;Pine & Lieven, 1997;Pine, Lieven & Rowland, 1998). In the literature, there has traditionally been a dichotomy between those who argue for lexically specific learning and those who argue that children are generalising across specific instances, extracting verb-and construction-general information about the regularities of their language from early on.…”
Section: Competing Constructions 26mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…where's X going), they will use these patterns in their production (see Pine et al, 1998) and they will be faster (Bannard & Matthews, 2008) and more accurate (Theakston, Lieven, Pine & Rowland, 2005;Rowland, 2007) at using them. However, this does not prevent the learner from generalising across them on the basis of shared commonalities in form and meaning, extracting relevant cues such as word order cues and applying them across construction boundaries.…”
Section: Competing Constructions 26mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The bulk of these studies is that young children, in contrast to older children, tend to re-use the ungrammatical structure modelled by the experimenter in their own productions, a finding which is interpreted as attesting to children's lack of productivity with word order. The constructivist framework proposes that word order develops from lexically specific schemas formed around frequent verbs, i.e., lexical units that are relatively consistently ordered (Pine et al, 1998), and only slowly generalizes, around 3 or 4 years old, to an abstract representation independent of the verbs.…”
Section: The Debatementioning
confidence: 99%