2021
DOI: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2019.01.004
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Common neural processes during action-stopping and infrequent stimulus detection: The frontocentral P3 as an index of generic motor inhibition

Abstract: The stop-signal task (SST) is used to study action-stopping in the laboratory. In SSTs, the P3 event-related potential following stop-signals is considered to be a neural index of motor inhibition. However, a similar P3 deflection is often observed following infrequent events in noninhibition tasks. Moreover, within SSTs, stop-signals are indeed infrequent events, presenting a systematic confound that hampers the interpretation of the stop-signal P3 (and other candidate neural indices of motor inhibition). The… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
59
0

Year Published

2021
2021
2025
2025

Publication Types

Select...
3
3
2

Relationship

3
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 53 publications
(68 citation statements)
references
References 69 publications
(61 reference statements)
3
59
0
Order By: Relevance
“…234 healthy adult humans (mean age: 22.7, SEM: .43, 137 female, 25 left-handed) from the Iowa City community participated in the study, either for course credit or for an hourly payment. 123 of those datasets were published as part of other studies, none of which focused on β-bursting [54,[61][62][63]. All procedures were approved by the local ethics committee at the University of Iowa (IRB #201511709).…”
Section: Participantsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…234 healthy adult humans (mean age: 22.7, SEM: .43, 137 female, 25 left-handed) from the Iowa City community participated in the study, either for course credit or for an hourly payment. 123 of those datasets were published as part of other studies, none of which focused on β-bursting [54,[61][62][63]. All procedures were approved by the local ethics committee at the University of Iowa (IRB #201511709).…”
Section: Participantsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The task was identical to the one described in [54,[61][62][63]. In short, trials began with a fixation cross (500ms duration), followed by a white left-or rightward arrow (Go-signal).…”
Section: Taskmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Alternatively, recent work has shown that the P3 on the stop-signal task shares common neural generators with the P3 elicited in tasks involving infrequent stimulus detection (Waller, Hazeltine, & Wessel, 2019;Wessel & Huber, 2019). If P3 indexes a stimulus detection process, the larger P3 on stop-success compared to stop-failure trials might simply index the successful detection of the stop signal on the former and/or poor detection on the later trial type.…”
Section: Manifestation Of Response Inhibition and The P3mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This notion has spurred a fundamental discussion about which parts of the neural cascade of activity following stop-signals reflect the attentional detection of an infrequent instructed signal to stop, and which reflect the motor inhibition process itself (Aron et al, 2014;Hampshire & Sharp, 2015;Swick & Chatham, 2014). In many studies that address this question, an inferential contrast is used in which brain activity following stop-signals is compared to brain activity following perceptually identical, infrequent, expected events that do not convey a 'stopping' instruction (Schmajuk et al, 2006;Dimoska & Johnstone, 2008;Hampshire et al, 2010;Boehler et al, 2010;Tabu et al, 2011;Dodds et al, 2011;Chatham et al, 2012;Erika-Florence et al, 2014;Bissett & Logan, 2014;Elchlepp et al, 2015;Lawrence et al, 2015;Verbruggen et al, 2010;Waller, Hazeltine, and Wessel, 2019). In other words, those studies employ a purportedly 'noninhibitory' control condition that resembles the design of our current Experiment 2, where a go-signal is followed by an infrequent expected event.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In fact, one of the most controversial questions in the recent stopsignal literature is which exact neural or psychological processes following stop-signals are related to the attentional detection of the infrequent stop-signal itself, and which are related to the actual implementation of motor inhibition (Verbruggen et al, 2010;Hampshire et al, 2010;Matzke et al, 2013). To address this question, many studies have utilized control tasks whose stimulus layout matches the stop-signal task (i.e., a go-signal is followed by an infrequent second signal) but with an instruction that does not involve outright action stopping (e.g., to press a second button after the original go-response or to ignore the second signal entirely, Hampshire et al, 2010;Dodds et al, 2011;Chatham et al, 2012;Erika-Florence et al, 2014;Waller et al, 2019). If such expected infrequent stimuli presented outside of a stop-signal task produced the same type of reactive, nonselective motor inhibition that is found after unexpected infrequent stimuli, it would invalidate the assumption that a contrast between a stop-signal and an infrequent-signal control task would isolate the inhibitory process that is found in the stop-signal task.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%