1976
DOI: 10.1016/0022-5371(76)90023-2
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Comments on Clark's “The language-as-fixed-effect fallacy”

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

0
111
0
3

Year Published

1980
1980
2014
2014

Publication Types

Select...
5
3
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 178 publications
(114 citation statements)
references
References 6 publications
0
111
0
3
Order By: Relevance
“…Although analyses using stimulus items as a random factor have been traditionally performed in psycholinguistic research, such analyses are actually not appropriate in cases, such as the present set of studies, in which stimulus items were not selected randomly, but were selected to control several criteria (Cohen, 1976;Hino & Lupker, 2000;Keppel, 1976;Raaijmakers, Schrijnemakers, & Gremmen, 1999; J. E. K. Smith, 1976;Wike & Church, 1976). Therefore, items analyses were not conducted in any of the experiments reported here.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…Although analyses using stimulus items as a random factor have been traditionally performed in psycholinguistic research, such analyses are actually not appropriate in cases, such as the present set of studies, in which stimulus items were not selected randomly, but were selected to control several criteria (Cohen, 1976;Hino & Lupker, 2000;Keppel, 1976;Raaijmakers, Schrijnemakers, & Gremmen, 1999; J. E. K. Smith, 1976;Wike & Church, 1976). Therefore, items analyses were not conducted in any of the experiments reported here.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…Although there is some debate about whether or not to treat stimulus items as a random factor in statistical analyses (Cohen, 1976;Hino & Lupker, 2000;Keppel, 1976;Raaijmakers, 2003;Raaijmakers, Schrijnemakers, & Gremmen, 1999;Smith, 1976;Wike & Church, 1976), it is the current practice in psycholinguistic research to conduct both types of analyses. For consistency with this convention, both types of analyses will be reported; however, the discussion and interpretation of the results will be based only on the analyses in which participants were treated as a random factor.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Because every task used in laboratory settings has advantages and disadvantages, replication across a variety of tasks increases our confidence that the observed effect was not due to the assumptions of a particular task employed in a particular experiment. Furthermore, Wike and Church (1976) recommended replication as a means of generalizing results without resorting to statistical techniques that might be inappropriate, such as analyses that treat stimulus items as a random factor.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The risk with treating items as random effects is that if the items were not sampled randomly from a population, as other researchers have argued is true for many types of language experiments, treating them as random will result in less statistical power, thereby creating a greater risk of false negative results (Wike and Church, 1976;Cohen, 1976;Keppel, 1976;Smith, 1976;Wickens and Keppel, 1983;Raaijmakers et al, 1999;Raaijmakers, 2003). Although we believe that the current experiments do not require items to be treated as random effects (because the different lexicalizations were not randomly sampled, but rather were instead carefully created to be representative of the conditions of interest, and because the items were lexically matched across conditions), we nonetheless constructed linear mixed effects models treating both participants and items as crossed random effects for the ME and LS experiments, and simulated p-values using the languageR package (Baayen, 2007;Baayen et al, 2008).…”
Section: Mixed Effects Modelsmentioning
confidence: 99%