2014
DOI: 10.3758/s13414-014-0737-9
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Combining attention: a novel way of conceptualizing the links between attention, sensory processing, and behavior

Abstract: Many everyday behaviors appear to require both the interpretation of incoming sensory information and the maintenance of a current task goal. This intuitive notion suggests that combining attentional control processes might reflect a fundamentally novel way in which attention supports complex behavior. Using an established paradigm, here we show that joint recruitment in multiple attention control systems leads to corresponding combined increases in behavior and underlying sensory processing of attended target… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
23
0

Year Published

2015
2015
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
5
1

Relationship

1
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 21 publications
(25 citation statements)
references
References 31 publications
2
23
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This result extends recent discussions concerning the importance of a cue's selection history in spatial orienting (Awh, Belopolsky, & Theeuwes, 2012;Ristic & Landry, 2015) and highlights the idea that the human attention system is sensitive to contextual, motivational, and learning factors. Note that because these theoretical accounts argue that attention is recruited differentially as a function of the incoming sensory information, it is reasonable to expect that other types of attentional cues that have traditionally been used to elicit and measure attention within a cuing task (e.g., peripheral onsets, nondirectional symbols) may affect attention in yet a different manner depending on their typical usage in life, prior selection history, and/or type of required attentional control.…”
Section: Sensory-specific Attentionsupporting
confidence: 86%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…This result extends recent discussions concerning the importance of a cue's selection history in spatial orienting (Awh, Belopolsky, & Theeuwes, 2012;Ristic & Landry, 2015) and highlights the idea that the human attention system is sensitive to contextual, motivational, and learning factors. Note that because these theoretical accounts argue that attention is recruited differentially as a function of the incoming sensory information, it is reasonable to expect that other types of attentional cues that have traditionally been used to elicit and measure attention within a cuing task (e.g., peripheral onsets, nondirectional symbols) may affect attention in yet a different manner depending on their typical usage in life, prior selection history, and/or type of required attentional control.…”
Section: Sensory-specific Attentionsupporting
confidence: 86%
“…Instead, the measure of attention is contaminated by the concurrent processes of tonic alertness and voluntary temporal preparation as well as by their interactions. We have shown here that the nature of those interactions depends on the characteristics of the attentional cue and the particular task settings, while existing data also indicate that those interactions may also differ as a function of the required response (e.g., target discrimination vs. target detection; Gabay & Henik, 2008, 2010Hayward & Ristic, 2013a;Ristic & Landry, 2015). Systematic investigations are needed to assess the contributions of tonic alertness and voluntary temporal preparation and their interactions to the measures of spatial orienting and the foreperiod effect elicited by the classic cuing task.…”
Section: Why No Dissociations Until Nowmentioning
confidence: 69%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…It may be that these attentional biases are based on a mixture of exogenous and endogenous processes. These combined effects lend themselves better to recent theorizing by Ristic and Landry (2015) and Goodhew, Kendall, Ferber, and Pratt (2014), who have produced experimental results that imply that endogenous and exogenous controls of attention interact flexibly and are engaged jointly by a single behaviorally relevant cue, resulting in an additive effect that leads to increased sensory processing of attended targets.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 56%
“…High trait anxiety is associated with high trait sensory sensitivity (Engel-Yeger and Dunn, 2011b), and central sensitisation, including those with NSCLBP (Franklin, 2014). A common link between anxiety and sensory sensitivity is the low threshold of sensitivity to stimuli (Ristic and Landry, 2015). Those with anxiety and high sensory sensitivity exhibit M A N U S C R I P T A C C E P T E D ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 7 physiological differences involving impaired inhibitory control mechanisms and impaired cognitive function (Ansari and Derakshan, 2011b), similar to people with central sensitisation (Latremoliere and Woolf, 2009;Nijs et al, 2010;Berryman et al, 2013).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%