2001
DOI: 10.1002/acp.725
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Cognitive style and learning: performance of adaptors and innovators in a novel dynamic task

Abstract: The in¯uence of cognitive style on performance in a novel dynamic task modelled on the task of controlling air traf®c was investigated. It was hypothesized that participants who preferred a more adaptive cognitive style would maintain better rule adherence and safe distances between aircraft in a low-®delity air traf®c control task than those who preferred a more innovative style. Problems were presented to participants using computer-based scenarios in screening, practice, and experimental trials. Performance… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
7
0

Year Published

2008
2008
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
5

Relationship

0
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 7 publications
(8 citation statements)
references
References 17 publications
1
7
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In the specific context of new ventures, Zacharakis et al (1999) find differences in the ways entrepreneurs attribute reasons for venture failure as compared to venture success. When individuals are compelled to engage in tasks that require them to act in ways that are inconsistent with their respective cognitive styles, coping strategies may be employed to restore "order" to these disconcerting scenarios (Pounds and Bailey, 2001). Thus, our general proposition that differences in cognitive style may influence the way intentions are updated to reflect realized competitive conditions is consistent with prior research.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 61%
“…In the specific context of new ventures, Zacharakis et al (1999) find differences in the ways entrepreneurs attribute reasons for venture failure as compared to venture success. When individuals are compelled to engage in tasks that require them to act in ways that are inconsistent with their respective cognitive styles, coping strategies may be employed to restore "order" to these disconcerting scenarios (Pounds and Bailey, 2001). Thus, our general proposition that differences in cognitive style may influence the way intentions are updated to reflect realized competitive conditions is consistent with prior research.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 61%
“…For example, Fuller and Kaplan (2004) examined the effect of style on the performance of auditors and found that analytic auditors performed better on analytic tasks than on intuitive tasks, with intuitive auditors showing the opposite pattern. Pounds and Bailey (2001) investigated the effects of style differences on performance of air-traffic-control tasks and reported that performance improved across experimental trials for those who had a KAI adaptor style but not for those who had a KAI innovator style. Pounds and Bailey interpreted their results as indicating that the innovators' style led them to disregard rules for successful performance, such that they were unable to sustain repetitious procedures for extended periods.…”
Section: Person-environment Fitmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…Linear scanlines are a quick and relatively simple way of systematically collecting fracture data (Agosta et al, 2010;Bigi et al, 2015;Chesnaux et al, 2009;Guerriero et al, 2011;Ortega et al, 2006;Tóth, 2010). This method was developed in rock engineering for a quantitative description of discontinuities in rock masses (Priest, 1993) and then adopted to describe natural fracture networks (Becker and Gross, 1996;Van Dijk et al, 2000;Newman, 2005;Peacock and Sanderson, 2018). The method involves laying out a tape measure on the outcrop and measuring both the number (N) and the attributes of fractures which intersect the scanline (e.g.…”
Section: Fracture Data Collection and Analysismentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The method involves laying out a tape measure on the outcrop and measuring both the number (N) and the attributes of fractures which intersect the scanline (e.g. orientation, spacing, length above and below the scanline, aperture, type of terminations, filling, or mineralisation) (Priest, 1993;Priest and Hudson, 1981). To fully sample a fracture network, multiple linear scanlines should be completed with different orientations, and the Terzaghi correction should be applied to reduce orientation bias (Mauldon and Mauldon, 1997;Terzaghi, 1965).…”
Section: Fracture Data Collection and Analysismentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation