1994
DOI: 10.1037/0735-7044.108.1.57
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Cognitive functions and aging in the dog: Acquisition of nonspatial visual tasks.

Abstract: Old, middle-aged, and young dogs were compared on discrimination and reversal learning and on acquisition of a delayed-nonmatching-to-sample (DNMS) test of recognition memory. DNMS acquisition was acquired more rapidly by young dogs. Reversal deficits were found between aged mixed-breed dogs and young beagles, but not between old and young beagles. Aged beagles also showed unexpected deficits in reward approach and object approach learning. Aged mixed-breed dogs did not show deficits in reward approach and obj… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
2

Citation Types

11
199
3
3

Year Published

2003
2003
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
4
3

Relationship

2
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 205 publications
(221 citation statements)
references
References 33 publications
11
199
3
3
Order By: Relevance
“…Our results indicate that cognitive function declines with age in dogs, but that the decline is domain, or task, specific and sensitive to previous experience. Procedural learning and memory, as well as simple discrimination learning, are generally insensitive to aging (Milgram et al 1994). By contrast, tests of executive function, such as discrimination reversal learning (Milgram et al 1994;Tapp et al 2003a), and working memory (Adams et al 2000b;Head et al 1995), such as a delayednonmatching-to-position (DNMP) task (Chan et al 2002), are highly sensitive to aging.…”
Section: Age-associated Cognitive Decline In Dogsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Our results indicate that cognitive function declines with age in dogs, but that the decline is domain, or task, specific and sensitive to previous experience. Procedural learning and memory, as well as simple discrimination learning, are generally insensitive to aging (Milgram et al 1994). By contrast, tests of executive function, such as discrimination reversal learning (Milgram et al 1994;Tapp et al 2003a), and working memory (Adams et al 2000b;Head et al 1995), such as a delayednonmatching-to-position (DNMP) task (Chan et al 2002), are highly sensitive to aging.…”
Section: Age-associated Cognitive Decline In Dogsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In other instances, age sensitivity depends on prior test experience. Naïve young and aged dogs do not differ in their ability to learn relatively simple size discrimination tasks (Milgram et al 1994). Once both groups have repeated experience on discrimination learning tasks, however, age differences emerge in that aged dogs require more trials to learn the task than do young dogs (Milgram 2003).…”
Section: Age-associated Cognitive Decline In Dogsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Pathological aging with behavioral changes has been described in aged dogs as Canine Svicero et al Cognitive Dysfunction Syndrome (CDS), which has clinical, imaging, and histopathological findings similar to human Alzheimer's disease (MILGRAM et al, 1994;CUMMINGS et al, 1996b, c). The main categories describing the clinical signs compatible with CDS is described by DISHA: disorientation; altered Interactions with people or other pets; Sleep-wake cycle alterations, House-soiling and altered Activity level (LANDESBERG et al, 2003).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Dogs have a shorter lifespan compared to humans; and therefore, the aging process is accelerated in these animals (LANDSBERG & RUEHL, 1997;HEIBLUM et al, 2007;HEAD, 2004). The behavioral changes of old dogs may be considered as part of the normal aging process, or may be attributed towards pathological aging caused by a brain disease that affects learning, motor activity and memory (MILGRAM et al, 1994;LANDSBERG & RUEHL, 1997;RUEHL & HART, 1998;ADAMS et al, 2000;CHAN et al, 2002).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%