1994
DOI: 10.1007/bf02244750
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Cognitive effects of milacemide and methylphenidate in healthy young adults

Abstract: Cognitive effects of the novel glycine prodrug milacemide (400 mg), the catecholaminergic agonist methylphenidate (20 mg), and placebo were evaluated in 48 healthy young adults. Throughout a 6-h drug treatment day, subjects repeatedly performed tests of target-detection vigilance, immediate and delayed verbal free recall, and Buschke Selective Reminding; total free recall and forced-choice recognition tests were administered at the end of the day. Significant improvement in both vigilance reaction time and Sel… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

7
37
1

Year Published

1998
1998
2015
2015

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 58 publications
(45 citation statements)
references
References 35 publications
7
37
1
Order By: Relevance
“…However, the evidence offered by most of the studies reporting improvements is not convincing. It has been reported that within studies, MPH had an effect on one sample (Galynker et al, 1997;Prendergast et al, 1998), one type of working memory task (Aman et al, 1991;Camp-Bruno & Herting, 1994), or one type of error (Mehta et al, 2000), but not on another. Camp-Bruno and Herting's report that MPH improved the learning of words, as compared with a placebo, was based on results that were significant at p , .10, which is usually considered not statistically significant.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 42%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…However, the evidence offered by most of the studies reporting improvements is not convincing. It has been reported that within studies, MPH had an effect on one sample (Galynker et al, 1997;Prendergast et al, 1998), one type of working memory task (Aman et al, 1991;Camp-Bruno & Herting, 1994), or one type of error (Mehta et al, 2000), but not on another. Camp-Bruno and Herting's report that MPH improved the learning of words, as compared with a placebo, was based on results that were significant at p , .10, which is usually considered not statistically significant.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 42%
“…It is possible that the apparent inconsistencybetween the present results and those in studies that have found that MPH improved memory in humans (Aman et al, 1991;Camp-Bruno & Herting, 1994;Galynker et al, 1997;Mehta et al, 2000;Rapport et al, 1993) is due to species differences between samples. However, the dopaminergic systems of pigeons and mammals are highly similar (Karle, Anderson, Medina, & Reiner, 1996), and the neostriatum caudolaterale of pigeons is believed to be equivalent to the mammalian prefrontal cortex (Hartmann & Gunturkun, 1998), an area that is involved in memory, attention, and the effects of MPH (Mehta et al, 2000;Puumala & Sirvio, 1998;Ragozzino, 2000).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 51%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Contrary to a preliminary encouraging study [89], milacemide (400 mg, single dose) did not improve cognitive performance in healthy adults, and was even associated with decreases in some vigilance performances [90]. The research on this substance was discontinued afterwards.…”
Section: Monoamine-oxidase Inhibitorssupporting
confidence: 44%
“…In spite of the widespread use of MPH as a cognitive enhancer, multiple cognitive tasks have not unequivocally demonstrated its efficacy in healthy adults. For example, although MPH enhanced vigilance in two studies [6,7], other research found this effect only under special conditions such as task novelty [8] or its challenging nature [9]. Conflicting data was found for the effect of MPH under conditions of sleep deprivation, enhancing vigilance in nine sleep deprived individuals [10] but ineffective in 20 other carefully selected students whether sleep deprived or not [11].…”
Section: Introductioncontrasting
confidence: 41%