2017
DOI: 10.1080/02699052.2017.1341645
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Cognitive correlates of pragmatic language comprehension in adult traumatic brain injury: A systematic review and meta-analyses

Abstract: Effective pragmatic comprehension of language is critical for successful communication and interaction, but this ability is routinely impaired following Traumatic Brain Injury (TBI) (1,2). Individual studies have investigated the cognitive domains associated with impaired pragmatic comprehension, but there remains little understanding of the relative importance of these domains in contributing to pragmatic comprehension impairment following TBI. This paper presents a systematic meta-analytic review of the obse… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

0
17
0
1

Year Published

2018
2018
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
4
3
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 37 publications
(19 citation statements)
references
References 63 publications
0
17
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…However, this injury pattern does have some differences from the patterns reported in mTBI. The prevalence of individuals presenting with two or more symptoms and the SVV abnormalities seems higher than one would expect after conventional mTBI . In addition, the low incidence of headaches (around 25%) is unusual, as many studies of mTBI show that headache is one of the most common and persistent symptoms .…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 96%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…However, this injury pattern does have some differences from the patterns reported in mTBI. The prevalence of individuals presenting with two or more symptoms and the SVV abnormalities seems higher than one would expect after conventional mTBI . In addition, the low incidence of headaches (around 25%) is unusual, as many studies of mTBI show that headache is one of the most common and persistent symptoms .…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…The prevalence of individuals presenting with two or more symptoms and the SVV abnormalities seems higher than one would expect after conventional mTBI. 23,24 In addition, the low incidence of headaches (around 25%) is unusual, as many studies of mTBI show that headache is one of the most common and persistent symptoms. [25][26][27] Perhaps the most important objective clinical feature is the nearly universal evidence of otolithic impairment; such uniformity in symptoms is uncommon in mTBI cases from other sources.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The term refers to goaldirected behaviours (Miyake et al 2000), including working memory, shifting and planning (Ashman, Gordon, Cantor & Hibbard, 2006;Dikmen et al, 2009;Smith & Jonides, 1999;Sullivan, Riccio & Castillo, 2009;Thomas, Snyder, Pietrzak & Maruff, 2014). The abovementioned abilities seem to have a significant role in the capacity to communicate efficiently, and they may be at least partially responsible for pragmatic impairment in TBI at different levels and to varying extents (McDonald & Pearce, 1998;Channon & Watts, 2003;Key-DeLyria & Altmann, 2016;Marini, Zettin & Galetto, 2014; for a meta-analysis see Rowley et al, 2017). Douglas (2010) investigated pragmatic-communication difficulties in TBI as well as difficulties related to the capacity to handle information and the speed of verbal processing.…”
Section: Relation Between Cognitive and Tom Abilities And Pragmatics In Individuals With Tbimentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Indeed, it is well established that deficits in ToM may result from a brain injury (for reviews, see Martin-Rodriguez & León-Carrión, 2010, McDonald, 2013and Weed, 2008. These deficits of theory of the mind, and particularly second-order ToM, co-occurred with difficulties in understanding irony in many ABI patients (Channon, Pellijeff & Rule, 2005;McDonald & Flanagan, 2004;Shamay-Tsoory, Tomer & Aharon-Peretz, 2005;Winner, Brownell, Happé, Blum & Pincus, 1998; for a review see Rowley et al,2017). Moreover, several studies using neuroimaging methods have shown the activation of a neural ToM network -left and right temporal-parietal junction, medial prefrontal cortex, and precuneus -when performing a task of irony understanding (Eviatar & Just, 2006;Shibata, Toyomura, Itoh & Abe, 2010;Spotorno, Koun, Prado, Van Der Henst & Running head: Heterogeneity of neuropsychological and pragmatics profiles Noveck, 2012;Uchiyama et al, 2006;Wakusawa et al, 2007).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%