1971
DOI: 10.1037/h0030954
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Cognitive components of separation anxiety.

Abstract: Each of twenty-four 11-month-old infants watched his mother leave him from an exit in the home that was either normally or rarely used by the mother. The incidence of crying, staring, and crawling to the exit was greater when the mother left by the unfamiliar exit suggesting that cognitive components are a factor in the phenomenon of separation anxiety.

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
8
0

Year Published

1977
1977
2011
2011

Publication Types

Select...
4
4
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 40 publications
(8 citation statements)
references
References 6 publications
0
8
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Negative responses may also occur when the attachment figure engages in unpredictable behavior. In one study (Littenberg, Tulkin, & Kagan, 1971), mother's disappearance into a closet-a discrepant event-led to distress, whereas her departure through a door did not do so. In addition, we know that 12-month-old infants can actl vely regulate their emotional states by averting their gaze from a stranger as they become alarmed (Waters, Matas, & Sroufe, 1975) or by alternating approaches to a stranger with retreats to the mother, depending on whether affiliative or wary feelings predominate (Bretherton & Ainsworth, 1974;Bronson, 1972).…”
Section: Communication Of Emotion In Preverbal Infantsmentioning
confidence: 94%
“…Negative responses may also occur when the attachment figure engages in unpredictable behavior. In one study (Littenberg, Tulkin, & Kagan, 1971), mother's disappearance into a closet-a discrepant event-led to distress, whereas her departure through a door did not do so. In addition, we know that 12-month-old infants can actl vely regulate their emotional states by averting their gaze from a stranger as they become alarmed (Waters, Matas, & Sroufe, 1975) or by alternating approaches to a stranger with retreats to the mother, depending on whether affiliative or wary feelings predominate (Bretherton & Ainsworth, 1974;Bronson, 1972).…”
Section: Communication Of Emotion In Preverbal Infantsmentioning
confidence: 94%
“…For example, Povinelli et al (1996) concluded that children younger than age 4 are unable to view themselves as an entity moving through time and cannot integrate past, present, and future versions of the self. Vasey (1993) proposes that even a vague notion of the near future, like that seen in children as young as 2 years old (Littenberg et al 1971), may be sufficient for worry. However, the increase in ability to understand the future seen in children around age 8 (Wallace and Rabin 1960) likely results in an ability to experience truly generalized worry.…”
Section: Cognitive Developmentmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…From this perspective, even toddlers may be capable of worrying. For example, 2-year-olds have been shown to be capable of entertaining hypotheses regarding the near-future probable actions of others (Littenberg, Tulkin, & Kagan, 1971), allowing anxiety about the possibility of separation. Similarly, Kagan (1981) has demonstrated that signs of anxiety can be seen in anticipation of task failure during this period.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%