2006
DOI: 10.1073/pnas.0607999103
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Cognitive cladistics and cultural override in Hominid spatial cognition

Abstract: Current approaches to human cognition often take a strong nativist stance based on Western adult performance, backed up where possible by neonate and infant research and almost never by comparative research across the Hominidae. Recent research suggests considerable cross-cultural differences in cognitive strategies, including relational thinking, a domain where infant research is impossible because of lack of cognitive maturation. Here, we apply the same paradigm across children and adults of different cultur… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1

Citation Types

6
148
0
2

Year Published

2007
2007
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
4
4

Relationship

2
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 180 publications
(164 citation statements)
references
References 49 publications
6
148
0
2
Order By: Relevance
“…When conversing, people are particularly likely (1) to encode and reason using the semantic tools provided by their language and (2) to honor the semantic distinctions of the language. Our findings are compatible with such a thinking-for-language account, although of course they do not rule out stronger effects of language in other contexts (see Haun, Rapold, Call, Janzen, & Levinson, 2006;Majid et al, 2004). Our findings here are less dramatic than those predicted by the strong Whorfian hypothesis, but they still leave room for pervasive semantic effects, given the prominence of language in human mental life.…”
Section: Thinking For Languagesupporting
confidence: 55%
“…When conversing, people are particularly likely (1) to encode and reason using the semantic tools provided by their language and (2) to honor the semantic distinctions of the language. Our findings are compatible with such a thinking-for-language account, although of course they do not rule out stronger effects of language in other contexts (see Haun, Rapold, Call, Janzen, & Levinson, 2006;Majid et al, 2004). Our findings here are less dramatic than those predicted by the strong Whorfian hypothesis, but they still leave room for pervasive semantic effects, given the prominence of language in human mental life.…”
Section: Thinking For Languagesupporting
confidence: 55%
“…Using this comparative approach [also sometimes called 'cognitive cladistics', (Haun, Rapold et al, 2006)], we may find some cognitive skills shared across all members of a phylogenetic family, in our case the great apes (Hominidae: orangutans, gorillas, bonobos, chimpanzees and humans), while others will only occur in a subset of these species. Where patterns of skill distribution match the known phylogenetic tree of relatedness, we will be able to trace the phylogenetic inheritance of these traits, thus gaining access to the likely evolutionary history of the relevant abilities.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…(Gentner, 2003;Loewenstein & Gentner, 2005;Rattermann & Gentner, 1998). Furthermore, cross-linguistic differences in spatial relational language and parallel differences in processing-preferences for spatial relations have served as further indicators for the interaction between relational language and thought (Bowerman & Choi, 2001;Haun, Rapold, Call, Janzen, & Levinson, 2006;Majid, Bowerman, Kita, Haun, & Levinson, 2004). None of these authors has stated that the ability for language was necessary for recognizing relational similarity, however, the late acquisition in children, the interrelation with language and the central function in so many human cognitive abilities has led researchers to propose that the ability to recognize relational similarity is extraordinarily pronounced in humans (Oden, Thompson, & Premack, 2001;Penn, Holyoak, & Povinelli, 2008;Premack, 1983;Thompson & Oden, 1995, 2000 if not even the one thing that ''makes us smart" (Gentner, 2003).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Left-right language has also been implicated in an implicit rulelearning task using a tabletop array (21). Speakers of a language that uses allocentric spatial expressions (e.g., "north of me") and great apes from a variety of species all found it more difficult to learn a table-top pattern that maintained egocentric spatial relations (e.g., to the participant's left) than allocentric ones (e.g., to the north).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%