2014
DOI: 10.1080/00140139.2014.899633
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Close to me: the effect of asymmetrical environments on spatial attention

Abstract: The ability to monitor critical information on displays can be affected by asymmetrical distractors. In many workplaces, a display may be placed alongside a wall. This study explored whether a wall placed to the left/right affects spatial attention. A weak, task-specific, attraction effect was observed for walls on the left.

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

0
10
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 16 publications
(10 citation statements)
references
References 42 publications
0
10
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The presentation of pre-bisected lines and the subsequent judgments of the length of the line's two segments is a common measure of spatial attentional biases (e.g., Harvey, Milner, & Roberts, 1995;McCourt & Jewell, 1999). The pre-bisected line stimuli in this study were modelled on McCourt (2001) and Nicholls et al (2014). Stimuli were presented individually against a uniformly grey background.…”
Section: Landmark Task Stimulimentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The presentation of pre-bisected lines and the subsequent judgments of the length of the line's two segments is a common measure of spatial attentional biases (e.g., Harvey, Milner, & Roberts, 1995;McCourt & Jewell, 1999). The pre-bisected line stimuli in this study were modelled on McCourt (2001) and Nicholls et al (2014). Stimuli were presented individually against a uniformly grey background.…”
Section: Landmark Task Stimulimentioning
confidence: 99%
“…To assess spatial asymmetries in the landmark task, a response bias was calculated for each condition by subtracting the number of left responses from the number of right responses and converting the difference to a percentage of the total number of trials (Nicholls et al, 2014). The bias scores could range from −100 to +100, with negative and positive scores indicating a bias towards the left and right side, respectively.…”
Section: Analysesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The leftward bias can be seen in tasks, such as line bisection, in which participants overestimate the relative length on the left (McCourt, 2001;McCourt & Jewell, 1999;Thomas, Castine, Loetscher, & Nicholls, 2015). A leftward overestimation is seen for a range of other discriminations, such as luminance discrimination (Nicholls, Bradshaw, & Mattingley, 1999), for visual search (Nicholls et al, 2014), and even for the left/right mental representations of stimuli (Loftus & Nicholls, 2012;Loftus, Nicholls, Mattingley, Chapman, & Bradshaw, 2009).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Presentation 17.0 software (Neurobehavioral Systems) was used to generate and display task stimuli and record participant responses from standard QWERTY-layout keyboards. The stimuli and procedure were adapted from those used by Nicholls et al (2014). Targets and distractors were distributed across an invisible grid, 20 cells wide x 6 cells high, located in the centre of the display, subtending a visual angle of 28.98°wide x 7.94°high with grid elements evenly spaced approximately 1.50°apart both horizontally and vertically.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%