1993
DOI: 10.3758/bf03213392
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Classical conditioning of jaw movements in the pigeon: Acquisition and response topography

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
6
0

Year Published

1994
1994
2004
2004

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 9 publications
(7 citation statements)
references
References 16 publications
(18 reference statements)
1
6
0
Order By: Relevance
“…It takes longer to handle the larger grain (Killeen, Cate, & Tran, 1993), and this change in consummatory topography may have been conditioned to the keypeck. Such a classical conditioning of the form of the consummatory response to the operant response was reported by Remy and Zeigler (1993;Ploog & Zeigler, 1996, found that response rates increased with grain size, but that may have been because magnitude of reinforcement [and thus a] also covaried with grain size in their study).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 70%
“…It takes longer to handle the larger grain (Killeen, Cate, & Tran, 1993), and this change in consummatory topography may have been conditioned to the keypeck. Such a classical conditioning of the form of the consummatory response to the operant response was reported by Remy and Zeigler (1993;Ploog & Zeigler, 1996, found that response rates increased with grain size, but that may have been because magnitude of reinforcement [and thus a] also covaried with grain size in their study).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 70%
“…The position-specific operant contingencies would affect key pecks, which involve directed movements towards the keys, but would not affect gapes, which could occur anywhere in the chamber. Some studies show, in fact, that pigeons' conditional key pecking is a compound response, dissociable into head transport (neck) and gape (jaw) components (Mallin & Delius, 1983;Remy & Zeigler, 1993). The design of our study does not allow strong conclusions to be drawn, but the findings may suggest a strategy for dissociating response-reinforcer from CS-US contingencies.…”
Section: Response Rates and Latenciesmentioning
confidence: 67%
“…The finding that conditional gape topography was controlled by pellet size extends the range of conditions known to produce similar effects. Topographical similarities between key pecks and consummatory responses develop in operant paradigms even when no specific response form is reinforced (LaMon & Zeigler, 1984;Spetch, Wilkie, & Skelton, 1981;Wolin, 1968;Woodruff & Williams, 1976) and in autoshaping studies with response-independent food or water delivery (Jenkins & Moore, 1973;LaMon & Zeigler, 1984Woodruff & Williams, 1976) even when movements towards the CS are prevented through head fixation (Remy & Zeigler, 1993).…”
Section: Response Topographymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The measures used to define probability and topography in this study (pecks and gapes) reflect the operation of two different effector systems (neck and jaw, respectively). It has been repeatedly demonstrated that the two components may be experimentally dissociated, and that each may be brought under the associative control of either stimulus-reinforcer or response-reinforcer contingencies (Allan & Zeigler, 1994;Deich, Allan, & Zeigler, 1988;Lucas, Vodraska, & Wasserman, 1979;Mallin & Delius, 1983;Remy & Zeigler, 1993). However, although such dissociation may be demonstrated under laboratory conditions, it is probably the case that the acquisition of distinct response forms with variable probabilities involves the operation, in parallel, of both response-reinforcer and stimulus-reinforcer contingencies (e.g., Balsam, Deich, & Hirose, 1992, p. 32).…”
Section: Effects Of Deprivation and Reversal Manipulations On Probabimentioning
confidence: 99%