2016
DOI: 10.1038/ncomms12176
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Chunking as the result of an efficiency computation trade-off

Abstract: How to move efficiently is an optimal control problem, whose computational complexity grows exponentially with the horizon of the planned trajectory. Breaking a compound movement into a series of chunks, each planned over a shorter horizon can thus reduce the overall computational complexity and associated costs while limiting the achievable efficiency. This trade-off suggests a cost-effective learning strategy: to learn new movements we should start with many short chunks (to limit the cost of computation). A… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4
1

Citation Types

4
86
0

Year Published

2017
2017
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
3
3
3

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 96 publications
(91 citation statements)
references
References 43 publications
4
86
0
Order By: Relevance
“…While inference is a useful tool in learning, so too is chunking, or the ability to combine fine-scale solutions into bigger sets of solutions. For example, one can combine a series of motor responses into a single response [140,152], thereby reducing the complexity of the required input–output mapping.…”
Section: Figurementioning
confidence: 99%
“…While inference is a useful tool in learning, so too is chunking, or the ability to combine fine-scale solutions into bigger sets of solutions. For example, one can combine a series of motor responses into a single response [140,152], thereby reducing the complexity of the required input–output mapping.…”
Section: Figurementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Animals have the remarkable ability of flexibly performing long and complex sequences of movements [42,13,52,28]. In humans, dance provides an illustration of this ability.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…These motor chunks could also be further organized into larger chunks or flexibly re-used to create different sequences (Sakai et al, 2003). Such hierarchical structure would greatly reduce computational cost for planning and executing long 30 motor sequences (Ramkumar et al, 2016). An alternative idea is that sequences are non-hierarchically represented as a continuous set of transition probabilities between neighbouring movements (Hunt and Aslin, 2001;Reber, 1967;Stadler, 1992;Verwey and Abrahamse, 2012).…”
Section: Introduction 20mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Rather than analysing the increases or decreases of spatially smoothed activity, representational fMRI analysis makes inferences based on the similarity (or dissimilarity) of multivariate activity patterns across multiple experimental conditions (Ban and 60 Welchman, 2015;Chikazoe et al, 2014;Ejaz et al, 2015;Kriegeskorte et al, 2008;Yokoi et al, 2018). One potential problem in applying this method to study the hierarchical organization of movement sequences is that the specific organisation of motor memories may be different across individuals (Jimenez et al, 2011;Ramkumar et al, 2016), and is often influenced (and hence confounded) by biomechanical 65 constraints (Koch and Hoffmann, 2000). To address this problem, we first established a new behavioural paradigm that allowed us to experimentally manipulate the structure of motor sequence representations.…”
Section: Introduction 20mentioning
confidence: 99%