2011
DOI: 10.1103/physrevstper.7.010103
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Choosing the right solution approach: The crucial role of situational knowledge in electricity and magnetism

Abstract: Novice problem solvers are rather sensitive to surface problem features, and they often resort to trial and error formula matching rather than identifying an appropriate solution approach. These observations have been interpreted to imply that novices structure their knowledge according to surface features rather than according to problem type categories. However, it may also be the case that novices do know problem types, but cannot map the problem at hand to a known type, because they fail to create a suffic… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

0
32
0
1

Year Published

2012
2012
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 33 publications
(37 citation statements)
references
References 54 publications
0
32
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Investigations of secondary students' career choices have understandably focused on social constructs such as selfefficacy as important influences on career choice (Savelsbergh, de Jong, & Ferguson-Hessler, 2011). For example, Bandura et al write that ''…socioeconomic, familial, academic, and self-referent influences operate in concert to shape children's career trajectories,'' (Bandura, Barbaranelli, Caprara, & Pastorelli, 2001, p. 198).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Investigations of secondary students' career choices have understandably focused on social constructs such as selfefficacy as important influences on career choice (Savelsbergh, de Jong, & Ferguson-Hessler, 2011). For example, Bandura et al write that ''…socioeconomic, familial, academic, and self-referent influences operate in concert to shape children's career trajectories,'' (Bandura, Barbaranelli, Caprara, & Pastorelli, 2001, p. 198).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Several studies have found that experts categorize physics problems according to the underlying principles, whereas novices are sensitive to the superficial surface features (such as the different objects present in the task) (Chi et al 1981;Savelsbergh et al 2011;Snyder 2000). What is more, Snyder (2000) showed that experts, intermediates, and novices all represented problems at multiple levels of abstraction, but that intermediates' and experts' representations were more principle-based than those of novices who were sensitive to the concrete features of the problems.…”
Section: Cognitive Aspects Of Constructive Modelingmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…It has also been suggested that teachers' demonstration of the modeling of problem solving processes or letting the students compare worked out solutions leads to learning of deep, structured knowledge (Richey and Nokes-Malach 2015;Rittle-Johnson and Star 2007;Savelsbergh et al 2011). It may also be that the sufficiently simplified epistemology of, for example, Hestenes's and Halloun's approaches as wells as their clear and comprehensive rules that lead from the theoretical principles to model construction contribute to their effectiveness by scaffolding the teachers and students alike.…”
Section: Cognitive Aspects Of Constructive Modelingmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The most prevalent method for studying how people form categories for physics problems is by using card-sorting tasks [13][14][15][16][17][18][19][20][21]. In most of these studies, textbook-style physics problems are printed onto index cards or listed on a piece of paper and study participants are asked to place the problems into groups based on solution similarity and to assign a name to each group.…”
Section: A Overview Of Research On Categorizationmentioning
confidence: 99%