1983
DOI: 10.1901/jeab.1983.40-225
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Choice for Aperiodic Versus Periodic Ratio Schedules: A Comparison of Concurrent and Concurrent‐chains Procedures

Abstract: Choice between mixed-ratio schedules, consisting of equiprobable ratios of 1 and 99 responses per reinforcement, and fixed-ratio schedules of food reinforcement was assessed by two commonly used procedures: concurrent schedules and concurrent-chains schedules. Rats were trained under concurrent fixed-ratio mixed-ratio schedules, in which both ratio schedules were simultaneously available, and under a concurrent-chains schedule, in which access to one of the mutually exclusive ratio schedules comprising the ter… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
13
0

Year Published

1986
1986
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 13 publications
(15 citation statements)
references
References 31 publications
2
13
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Several non-human experiments have shown significant preference for the variable alternative over the fixed alternative when the two are equated (Fantino, 1967, Sherman & Thomas, 1968; Cicerone, 1976; Rider, 1983). In generalizing the results to humans, the experiment sought to discover what contingencies facilitate these patterns in choice behavior.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Several non-human experiments have shown significant preference for the variable alternative over the fixed alternative when the two are equated (Fantino, 1967, Sherman & Thomas, 1968; Cicerone, 1976; Rider, 1983). In generalizing the results to humans, the experiment sought to discover what contingencies facilitate these patterns in choice behavior.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Much work has been done with humans on decision-making under uncertainty, for example Prospect-Theory (Kahnemann & Tversky, 1979) while behavioral studies have addressed this issue with non-human subjects (Fantino, 1967; Sherman & Thomas, 1968; Rider, 1983). While there is no standard definition, the term “risky choice” in the current experiment is empirically defined as a choice between two options, one fixed and the other variable (in terms of the reinforcement schedule parameters, not reinforcer amount).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Sample trials were followed by choice trials, during which both sets of white lights were illuminated, and consequences of associated with both levers were available. For choice trials, a single response on either lever darkened the lights on the opposite lever to prevent switching (e.g., Rider 1983), and responses to that lever were recorded but had no other programmed consequences.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In the earliest demonstration of this effect, Hermstein (1964) asked pigeons to choose between an outcome providing food reward on a fixed-interval (FI) 15-sec schedule in which reward occurred for the first response after 15-sec had elapsed and an outcome providing food on a variable-interval (VI) 15-sec schedule in which reward occurred for the first response after a variable period of time, averaging 15-sec. Herrnstein Such risk-preference with reinforcer-delay risk has been found despite differences in procedures, reinforcement schedules, types of reinforcement and the species under investigation (Aheam et al, 1992;Kacelnik, 1995, 1997;Case et al, 1995;Cicerone, 1976;Davison, 1969Davison, , 1972Frankel and Vom Saal, 1976;Gibbon et al, 1988;Hursh and Fantino, 1973;Kendall, 1987Kendall, , 1989Killeen, 1968;Logan, 1965;Morris, 1986;Navarick and Fantino, 1975;Pubols, 1962;Rider, 1983;Sherman and Thomas, 1968;Zabludoff et al, 1988).…”
Section: Choosing Between Certain and Variable Outcomesmentioning
confidence: 99%