1976
DOI: 10.1901/jeab.1976.25-75
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

CHOICE AND THE RATE OF PUNISHMENT IN CONCURRENT SCHEDULES1

Abstract: Rats' responses on two levers were reinforced according to independent random-interval 1.5-min food schedules. In addition, both lever presses were intermittently punished according to several concurrent random-interval random-interval shock schedules. For the left, the scheduled rate of punishment was kept constant according to a random-interval 6-min schedule. For the right, the rate of punishment varied. As the frequency of punishment for the right lever press increased, its rate decreased. The rate of the … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

4
57
2
3

Year Published

1978
1978
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 49 publications
(66 citation statements)
references
References 20 publications
(31 reference statements)
4
57
2
3
Order By: Relevance
“…Given the exclusive responding, the bias due to this stimulus could not be quantified by the generalized matching law (being infinite). Deluty (1976), using rats working under concurrent schedules of foodmaintained responding, did not find exclusive responding as long as responses on both levers randomly produced shock. However, when shock production was limited to one lever, the rats responded almost exclusively on the other lever.…”
mentioning
confidence: 80%
“…Given the exclusive responding, the bias due to this stimulus could not be quantified by the generalized matching law (being infinite). Deluty (1976), using rats working under concurrent schedules of foodmaintained responding, did not find exclusive responding as long as responses on both levers randomly produced shock. However, when shock production was limited to one lever, the rats responded almost exclusively on the other lever.…”
mentioning
confidence: 80%
“…In contrast, Equation 2b predicts R1 suppression because of an increase in its denominator; more specifically, that portion of the denominator which comprises the absolute value of alternative 2. An additional consequence of this suppression of R1 for both sets of equations is an increase in response rate to the nopunishment alternative (i.e., punishment-contrast: Azrin & Holz, 1966;Deluty, 1976 , however, predicts decreases in R2 because of a proportionally greater increase in the numerator, i.e., a direct increase in the absolute, and relative, value of alternative 2 events. The divergent predictions made by the two sets of equations are of greater interest.…”
Section: R1=kmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A different combination rule for punishment and reinforcement has recently been offered by Deluty (1976) and Deluty and Church (1978). In this model, punishment of a response can be interpreted as diminishing the relative value of associated events in an indirect manner, through an increase in the absolute value for alternative responses.…”
Section: R1=kmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…In Figure 4, the absolute response rate dur- (Brethower and Reynolds, 1962;Terrace, 1968) and concurrent schedules (Deluty, 1976). Working with rats in a concurrent variable-interval variable-interval food and shock schedule, Deluty (1976) Before attempting to modify Equation 9 to account for punishment-produced contrast, the reliability of this phenomenon still has to be established (Bouzas, 1976;Dinsmoor, 1952;Rachlin, 1966).…”
Section: The Connected Points Inmentioning
confidence: 99%