2019
DOI: 10.1017/s0305000919000187
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Chinese preschoolers’ acquisition of temporal adverbs indicating past, present, and future: a corpus-based study

Abstract: This study investigated the development of temporal adverbs in early childhood Mandarin. All cases of temporal adverbs indicating the past, present, and future were extracted from the Early Child Mandarin Corpus (168 children in four age groups: 2;6, 3;6, 4;6, 5;6). Data analyses indicated: (1) Mandarin-speaking children produced a repertoire of 21 types of temporal adverbs, and the children in the first age group (M = 2;6) were capable of using temporal adverbs to denote past, present, and future events; (2) … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
2

Citation Types

0
0
0

Year Published

2021
2021
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
2
2
2

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 19 publications
(17 citation statements)
references
References 30 publications
0
0
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The development of temporal adverbs has been described for a number of languages (e.g., Ames, 1946;Chejnová, 2017;Liang et al, 2019;Parm, 2013;Surakka, 2019;Weist, 1989;Weist & Buczowska, 1987). This body of research suggests that temporal adverbs emerge in children's language between the ages of 2-3 years, depending on the language.…”
Section: The Development Of Temporal Adverbs In Child Languagementioning
confidence: 99%
See 4 more Smart Citations
“…The development of temporal adverbs has been described for a number of languages (e.g., Ames, 1946;Chejnová, 2017;Liang et al, 2019;Parm, 2013;Surakka, 2019;Weist, 1989;Weist & Buczowska, 1987). This body of research suggests that temporal adverbs emerge in children's language between the ages of 2-3 years, depending on the language.…”
Section: The Development Of Temporal Adverbs In Child Languagementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Given that research shows that adverbs are often polysemous (Hakulinen & Saari, 1995) and that the meaning of the abstract concepts can change depending on the context (Lebois et al, 2015) we assume that our target adverbs JO and KOHTA can also have multiple meanings (see examples (1) -(3) above). The only previous studies that we are aware of that consider discursive meanings in children's adverb use are Liang et al (2019) and Surakka (2019). Liang et al (2019) studied the acquisition of temporal adverbs in Mandarin Chinese, and suggested that by varying the positions of temporal adverbs in utterances, it is possible either to highlight certain meaning components or to constitute a discourse function concerning an ongoing action (e.g., ).…”
Section: The Development Of Temporal Adverbs In Child Languagementioning
confidence: 99%
See 3 more Smart Citations