2012
DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01850.x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Children Trust a Consensus Composed of Outgroup Members—But Do Not Retain That Trust

Abstract: Children prefer to learn from informants in consensus with one another. However, no research has examined whether this preference exists across cultures, and whether the race of the informants impacts that preference. In 2 studies, one hundred thirty-six 4- to 7-year-old European American and Taiwanese children demonstrated a systematic preference for a consensus. Nevertheless, the initial strength and persistence of that preference depended on the racial composition of the consensus. Children's preference for… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

8
109
0

Year Published

2015
2015
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
3
3
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 94 publications
(121 citation statements)
references
References 70 publications
8
109
0
Order By: Relevance
“…While previous studies on object labeling have found that consensus preference is more pronounced when the race of the consensus group matches that of the child (Chen, et al, 2011(Chen, et al, , 2013, we did not make a specific prediction about the role of the race of dissenter in this context given that we did not have a specific prediction regarding children's decisions in the context of object labeling.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 64%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…While previous studies on object labeling have found that consensus preference is more pronounced when the race of the consensus group matches that of the child (Chen, et al, 2011(Chen, et al, , 2013, we did not make a specific prediction about the role of the race of dissenter in this context given that we did not have a specific prediction regarding children's decisions in the context of object labeling.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 64%
“…Although studies with chil-dren are far less numerous than studies with adult participants, recent research has shown that young children, too, respond to consensus testimony. For example, young children rely on information from three adults in consensus rather than a lone dissenting adult regarding the correct name for a novel object (Chen, Corriveau, & Harris, 2013;Corriveau, Fusaro, & Harris, 2009;Fusaro & Harris, 2008). Likewise, related work on other conventional norms like forms of greeting, modes of dress, or classroom rules, has revealed that young children draw on social consensus when evaluating these rules, the reasons for them, and the consequences of violating such conventional norms (see Smetana, Jambon, & Ball, 2014, for a review).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…It is possible that there may be some wrong belief with a high prior probability that unknowledgeable informants could converge on. * We also modeled the results of Chen et al (2012) which reproduced the pretest (group) trials of Corriveau, Fusaro, and Harris (2009) with different age groups. The model procedure was identical.…”
Section: Consensus Corriveau Fusaro and Harris (2009) Looked At Chmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Similarly, people learn differently from informants with different features. To list a few of the documented phenomenon, people prefer informants who: label common objects accurately (Pasquini, Corriveau, Koenig, & Harris, 2007;Koenig & Harris, 2005;Fitneva & Dunfield, 2010), produce more understandable errors (Einav & E. J. Robinson, 2010;Kondrad & Jaswal, 2012), and are part of an agreeing majority (Corriveau, Fusaro, & Harris, 2009;Chen, Corriveau, & Harris, 2012). Here we outline a probabilistic model that learns from and about informants of differing epistemic qualities which we shall then employ to explain empirical findings.…”
Section: Epistemic Trust Model Specificationmentioning
confidence: 99%