2013
DOI: 10.1080/15248372.2011.638686
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Children's Use of Social Categories in Thinking About People and Social Relationships

Abstract: A series of studies investigated White U.S. three- and four-year-old children’s use of gender and race information to reason about their own and others’ relationships and attributes. Three-year-old children used gender- but not race-based similarity between themselves and others to decide with whom they wanted to be friends, as well as to determine which children shared their own preferences for various social activities. Four-year-old (but not younger) children attended to gender and racial category membershi… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

7
171
1
3

Year Published

2013
2013
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
2
1

Relationship

2
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 173 publications
(189 citation statements)
references
References 47 publications
(70 reference statements)
7
171
1
3
Order By: Relevance
“…In a typical sorting task, children are asked to generate piles of pictures of people who “go together”; in a typical social preference task, children are asked to select potential playmates from displays that present a boy and a girl, or a White child and a Black child; and in a typical inference task, children are asked to think about which people (e.g., two boys, or a boy and girl) share hidden properties or relationships with one another. Children use visual information about gender and race to guide their responses in all these tasks at least as early as the preschool years (e.g., Aboud, 1988; Bigler & Liben, 1993; Kowalski & Lo, 2001; Ramsey & Myers, 1990; Shutts, Roben, & Spelke, 2013; Waxman, 2010; Yee & Brown, 1994). …”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In a typical sorting task, children are asked to generate piles of pictures of people who “go together”; in a typical social preference task, children are asked to select potential playmates from displays that present a boy and a girl, or a White child and a Black child; and in a typical inference task, children are asked to think about which people (e.g., two boys, or a boy and girl) share hidden properties or relationships with one another. Children use visual information about gender and race to guide their responses in all these tasks at least as early as the preschool years (e.g., Aboud, 1988; Bigler & Liben, 1993; Kowalski & Lo, 2001; Ramsey & Myers, 1990; Shutts, Roben, & Spelke, 2013; Waxman, 2010; Yee & Brown, 1994). …”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Although people can be categorized in many ways, we generally expect social groups to be characterized by affiliation between their members rather than solely by shared attributes (29). It is an open question whether infants under a year of age can represent such social relationships and use cues to affiliation to define social groups that can then be used in the service of inference about their members.…”
Section: Significancementioning
confidence: 99%
“…As might be expected, similarity in appearance and attitudes plays a large role in affiliative evaluations (birds of a feather flock together; e.g., Byrne, 1971;Sunnafrank, 1983), but so do more arbitrary or accidental properties. To illustrate, 3-to 6-year-olds prefer children of their own gender when asked with whom they want to be friends (e.g., Shutts, Pemberton Roben, & Spelke, 2013); they prefer children who have experienced a lucky event (e.g., found money on a sidewalk) over children who have experienced an unlucky event (e.g., gotten splashed by a passing car) (e.g., Olson, Dunham, Dweck, Spelke, & Banaji, 2008); and, in a phenomenon called evaluative contagion, they prefer associates of lucky children over those of unlucky children (e.g., Olson, Banaji, Dweck, & Spelke, 2006).…”
Section: Sociomoral Versus Affiliative Evaluations When It Comes Tomentioning
confidence: 99%