1988
DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1988.tb01474.x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Children's Understanding of Informational Access as Source of Knowledge

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

10
168
3
3

Year Published

2000
2000
2016
2016

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 115 publications
(184 citation statements)
references
References 14 publications
10
168
3
3
Order By: Relevance
“…Performance on the indirect access (Hear and Infer) type of the items was shown to improve over time, a finding in line with previous literature claiming an asymmetry in the development of different types of sources of information. According to this literature, visual perception is the most readily source accessible to young children, with verbal information following visual perception and inference being the most complex and least accessible information source for the young child (Wimmer & Hogrefe, 1988). The comparison of Experiments 1 and 2 revealed an asymmetry between children's linguistic command of evidential morphology and the ability to report non-linguistic knowledge sources, with non-linguistic source monitoring preceding the acquisition of linguistic evidentiality.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Performance on the indirect access (Hear and Infer) type of the items was shown to improve over time, a finding in line with previous literature claiming an asymmetry in the development of different types of sources of information. According to this literature, visual perception is the most readily source accessible to young children, with verbal information following visual perception and inference being the most complex and least accessible information source for the young child (Wimmer & Hogrefe, 1988). The comparison of Experiments 1 and 2 revealed an asymmetry between children's linguistic command of evidential morphology and the ability to report non-linguistic knowledge sources, with non-linguistic source monitoring preceding the acquisition of linguistic evidentiality.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Three-and 4-year olds have been reported to have difficulty understanding that a person who has been verbally informed about the contents of a box has informational access and would know what is in the box (Wimmer & Hogrefe, 1988;cf. Pratt & Bryant, 1990).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Studies looking at children's ability to explicitly identify the evidence for their beliefs generally conclude that young children are unable to at encode source information the time of experiencing an event; furthermore, the problem is more specific than simple memory limitations (Gopnik & Graf, 1988;O'Neill & Chong, 2001;O'Neill & Gopnik, 1991;Pillow, 1989;Povinelli & de Blois, 1992;Wimmer, Hogrefe, & Perner, 1988;Wimmer, Hogrefe, & Sodian, 1988;Woolley & Bruell, 1996). Typically, in these tasks, children discover the contents of a container through a single type of source (e.g.…”
Section: The Development Of Source Monitoringmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Figure 1 offers a metaphorical depiction of these two levels of understanding visual perception. Other research has confirmed and extended these findings by demonstrating that 4-to 5 year-olds, but not younger children, appear to understand that visual perception causes internal knowledge states in both the self and others (Wimmer, Hogrefe & Perner, 1988;Gopnik & Graf, 1988;Perner & Ogden, 1988;Povinelli & deBlois, 1992;Ruffman & Olson, 1989;O'Neill & Gopnik, 1991;O'Neill, Astington & Flavell, 1992). 2 Having previously obtained largely unconvincing results in studies examining chimpanzees' understanding of the connection between seeing and knowing (see Section V), we turned our attention to what (in humans at least) appears to be a less sophisticated (or …”
mentioning
confidence: 69%