2015
DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2014.05.002
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Children's recall of generic and specific labels regarding animals and people

Abstract: Although children tend to categorize objects at the basic level, we hypothesized that generic sentences would direct children’s attention to different levels of categorization. We tested children’s and adults’ short-term recall (Study 1) and longer-term recall (Study 2) for labels presented in generic sentences (e.g., Kids like to play jimjam) versus specific sentences (e.g., This kid likes to play jimjam). Label content was either basic level (e.g., cat, boy) or superordinate (e.g., animal, kid). As predicted… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1

Citation Types

2
12
0

Year Published

2015
2015
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

3
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 13 publications
(14 citation statements)
references
References 37 publications
2
12
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Furthermore, preschoolers treat properties conveyed in generic NPs as more conceptually central to categories than properties conveyed in nongeneric NPs (e.g., Cimpian & Markman, 2009; Gelman, Ware, & Kleinberg, 2010; Hollander, Gelman, & Raman, 2009). Finally, preschoolers are more apt to remember generic information versus specific information (e.g., Cimpian & Erickson, 2012; Gelman & Raman, 2007; Gülgöz & Gelman, 2015). …”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Furthermore, preschoolers treat properties conveyed in generic NPs as more conceptually central to categories than properties conveyed in nongeneric NPs (e.g., Cimpian & Markman, 2009; Gelman, Ware, & Kleinberg, 2010; Hollander, Gelman, & Raman, 2009). Finally, preschoolers are more apt to remember generic information versus specific information (e.g., Cimpian & Erickson, 2012; Gelman & Raman, 2007; Gülgöz & Gelman, 2015). …”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The narrator named the three characteristics (i.e., “This one likes …”). A memory cue sheet with the 12 previously-learned about characteristics eliminated memory demands and ensured that generic language did not result in increased recall for the presented information (Gülgöz & Gelman, 2015; see Figure 4). To reiterate, there were no pre-determined Zuttles/non-citizens in the categorization task.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We investigated these preferences for explanation generality in biology and physics across three age groups: 5-to 7-year-olds, 11-to 13-year-olds, and adults. Five-to 7-year-olds clearly differentiate between specific and generic language (e.g., Cimpian & Cadena, 2010;Gelman et al, 2002;Graham et al, 2011), show reliable explanatory preferences (particularly for explanatory breadth; Johnston et al, 2017;Samarapungavan, 1992), and conceptualize animal and artifact categories differently (e.g., Gelman, 1988;Rhodes & Gelman, 2009), making them an appropriate younger age group for the current investigation. However, given that some research has shown that children do not begin to show adult-like explanatory preferences until age 11 (Samarapungavan, 1992), we also included a group of 11-to 13-year-olds and a group of adults in our sample.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Second, and most crucially for the current study, children expect that generic information will extend more widely than token-level information. Even 2year-olds, when introduced to a novel property, are more likely to infer that the property extends to other category members when learning from generic language (e.g., "Blicks drink milk") than specific, token-level language (e.g., "This blick drinks milk"; Graham, Nayer, & Gelman, 2011;see also, Chambers, Graham, & Turner, 2008). In fact, by age 4, children are sensitive to even more nuanced distinctions in the relation between language and scope, inferring that universally quantified noun phrases (e.g., "All bears") extend more widely than generics (e.g., "Bears"; Brandone, Gelman, & Hedglen, 2015;Gelman, Leslie, Was, & Koch, 2015;Gelman et al, 2002;Hollander, Gelman, & Star, 2002).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation