2009
DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2009.01.014
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Children’s multiplicative transformations of discrete and continuous quantities

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

9
60
1

Year Published

2010
2010
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 54 publications
(76 citation statements)
references
References 45 publications
9
60
1
Order By: Relevance
“…It might be argued that the distance effect found in Experiment 1 is due to range-based analysis (Barth, Baron, Spelke, & Carey, 2009). The use of only 2D × 2D multiplication problems enabled participants to figure that the exact answers for all problems ranged from 500 to 10,000, and this is why any response to a reference number larger than 10,000 or smaller than 500 was fast.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 97%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…It might be argued that the distance effect found in Experiment 1 is due to range-based analysis (Barth, Baron, Spelke, & Carey, 2009). The use of only 2D × 2D multiplication problems enabled participants to figure that the exact answers for all problems ranged from 500 to 10,000, and this is why any response to a reference number larger than 10,000 or smaller than 500 was fast.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 97%
“…This pattern is adaptive because in such cases the sense of magnitude strategy could not guarantee a correct response. It suggests that adults approach this task with a rough idea for the magnitudes of multi-digit multiplication problems based on their ANS (Barth et al, 2009;McCrink & Spelke, 2010). When faced with an estimation comparison problem, they first attempt to solve it using their intuitive sense of magnitude based on the ANS.…”
Section: Strategy Analysismentioning
confidence: 99%
“…As examples, 6-year-olds can choose the game with the greater probability of winning when this is determined by a ratio of two continuous quantities (i.e., a doughnut divided into a red part and a blue part) (Jeong et al, 2007), and 4-year-olds are able to perform simple operations on fractional amounts or to match figures according to visuospatial ratios (Mix et al, 1999;Sophian, 2000). Barth, Baron, Spelke, and Carey (2009) showed that 6-and 7-year-olds are able to apply multiplicative transformations to discrete quantities and, in particular, to represent the half of a set and compare it with a third set. Their performance varies with the ratio between the to-be-compared quantities, suggesting that children operate on mental magnitudes.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…One possible reason may be that they promote a different understanding of how proportional components should be scaled. Scaling can be defined as a process of transforming absolute magnitudes while conserving relational properties, and it is therefore an important aspect of proportional reasoning (Barth, Baron, Spelke, & Carey, 2009;Boyer & Levine, 2012;McCrink & Spelke, 2010). The importance of scaling for proportional reasoning is evident in everyday life, for instance when one wants to adjust the amounts of ingredients for a cake for 4 people to 6 people, or prepare the same concentrations of syrup-water mixtures in different jugs.…”
Section: Such Findings Documenting Children's Difficulties With Fractmentioning
confidence: 99%