2007
DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2006.12.006
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Children’s metamemorial judgments in an event recall task

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

6
60
1
2

Year Published

2013
2013
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5
3

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 64 publications
(71 citation statements)
references
References 42 publications
6
60
1
2
Order By: Relevance
“…La memorización estratégica se encuentra entre las capacidades de control cognitivo más importantes (De Marie & Ferron, 2003;Siegler, 2004), esta capacidad requiere la implementación de esfuerzo mental y de recursos cognitivos dirigidos a mejorar el desempeño de la capacidad de memorización-aprendizaje (Bjorklund, Dukes & Douglas, 2009). Además del control ejecutivo se requiere de la capacidad de metacognición (Roebers, von der Linden, & Howie, 2007). El control metacognitivo se ejerce a partir del conocimiento de las capacidades propias -monitoreo- (Nelson & Narens, 1994); de esta forma los procesos cognitivos (lenguaje, memoria, pensamiento, etc.)…”
Section: Memoria Estratégicaunclassified
“…La memorización estratégica se encuentra entre las capacidades de control cognitivo más importantes (De Marie & Ferron, 2003;Siegler, 2004), esta capacidad requiere la implementación de esfuerzo mental y de recursos cognitivos dirigidos a mejorar el desempeño de la capacidad de memorización-aprendizaje (Bjorklund, Dukes & Douglas, 2009). Además del control ejecutivo se requiere de la capacidad de metacognición (Roebers, von der Linden, & Howie, 2007). El control metacognitivo se ejerce a partir del conocimiento de las capacidades propias -monitoreo- (Nelson & Narens, 1994); de esta forma los procesos cognitivos (lenguaje, memoria, pensamiento, etc.)…”
Section: Memoria Estratégicaunclassified
“…These naïve theories and the underlying heuristic rules are rarely, if ever, verbalized or deliberately employed, but their effects on memory performance are equal to those of explicit strategies (Dodson & Schacter, 2001;Ghetti, 2008;Howe, 2008;Meeks, Knight, Brewer, Cook, & Marsh, 2014). However, although researchers studying adult metacognition have long placed a heavy emphasis on the study of these heuristic rules, research on metacognition in children has only recently started to pay greater attention to the contribution made by these heuristic-based judgments (e.g., Koriat, Ackerman, Lockl, & Schneider, 2009a;Lockl & Schneider, 2002;Roebers, von der Linden, Schneider, & Howie, 2007). Generally, the results of these studies indicate that memory decisions are already based on some heuristics (e.g., the memorizing-effort or availability heuristic) by the age of 7 to 8 years.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…As a result, we obtained sufficient CJ for both correct and incorrect recall. In analogue to previous studies (Roebers, 2002;Roebers et al, 2007), we compared the average CJ given after correct recall with the average CJ given after incorrect recall. Results from the analysis of variance (ANOVA) revealed that children gave significantly higher CJ for correctly recognized items than for incorrect items at time 1, F (1,110) = 90.35, p < .001, ηp 2 = .37 as well at time 2, F (1,110) = 55.50, p < .001, ηp 2 = .34.…”
Section: Descriptive Analysismentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Classical measures of monitoring are judgments of learning (JOL), used in self-paced learning paradigms (Lockl & Schneider, 2003), and confidence judgments (CJ) for monitoring performance retrospectively (Pressley, Levin, Ghatala, & Ahmad, 1987). For self-regulated learning, CJ are assumed to be essential (Kleitman & Moscrop, 2010), and they have been shown to be a more sensitive indicator of early metacognitive monitoring skills than JOL (Roebers, von der Linden, Schneider, & Howie, 2007). The present paper will therefore focus on young children's CJ as indicator of their monitoring skills.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation