2010
DOI: 10.1348/135532509x474822
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Children's eyewitness identification performance: Effects of a Not Sure response option and accuracy motivation

Abstract: Purpose. Children who witness crimes are sometimes asked to view a photospread lineup to see if they can identify the culprit. Here, we examined the effectiveness of two manipulations designed to assist in overcoming the tendency of child eyewitnesses to choose from lineups, a tendency that manifests in false identifications from target‐absent lineups. Methods. Children aged around 12 years (N = 432) were randomly assigned to control, Not Sure, or accuracy motivation conditions in order to examine both targe… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

1
45
0

Year Published

2011
2011
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 29 publications
(46 citation statements)
references
References 30 publications
1
45
0
Order By: Relevance
“…It is widely documented that variability in encoding and test conditions is crucial when trying to detect reliable and generalizable effects (Brewer, Keast, & Sauer, 2010;Lindsay, Read, & Sharma, 1998 Evidence, 1999;Wells, 1993).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It is widely documented that variability in encoding and test conditions is crucial when trying to detect reliable and generalizable effects (Brewer, Keast, & Sauer, 2010;Lindsay, Read, & Sharma, 1998 Evidence, 1999;Wells, 1993).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Therefore, the difficulty in encoding unfamiliar faces in the first place, independent of any memory-related problems, may contribute to the eyewitness identification problems in children. This is important as researchers have already attempted to improve the reliability of eyewitness identifications in children by implementing procedural changes such as elimination lineups [12] or the inclusion of a "not sure" response option [13]. Therefore, any attempts to improve the accuracy of eyewitness identification in children also need to focus on assessing their ability to encode unfamiliar faces in the first place.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, it is difficult to generalize from Clake, Howell, and Davey's (2008) results since the studies they used in their comparisons did not experimentally manipulate the availability of the don't know option A more important question is whether a not sure option affects accuracy. Indeed, only a few line-up studies (e.g., Brewer, Keast, & Sauer, 2010;Steblay & Philips, 2011;Weber & Perfect, 2011) have directly manipulated the availability of the don't know or not sure option to examine its impact on witness identification accuracy using Koriat and Goldsmith's (1996) model as a theoretical framework. In Brewer, Keast and Sauer (2010), participants (children) viewed video clips of simulated crimes varying in terms of crime nature and exposure time.…”
Section: Applying Monitoring and Control To A Line-up Identification mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Indeed, only a few line-up studies (e.g., Brewer, Keast, & Sauer, 2010;Steblay & Philips, 2011;Weber & Perfect, 2011) have directly manipulated the availability of the don't know or not sure option to examine its impact on witness identification accuracy using Koriat and Goldsmith's (1996) model as a theoretical framework. In Brewer, Keast and Sauer (2010), participants (children) viewed video clips of simulated crimes varying in terms of crime nature and exposure time. After viewing the stimulus video, participants were randomly assigned to one of three instruction conditions and administered a lineup.…”
Section: Applying Monitoring and Control To A Line-up Identification mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation