2011
DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01101.x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Children’s essentialist reasoning about language and race

Abstract: Across four studies, we directly compared children's essentialist reasoning about the stability of race and language throughout an individual's lifespan. Monolingual English-speaking children were presented with a series of images of children who were either White or Black; each face was paired with a voice clip in either English or French. Participants were asked which of two adults each target child would grow up to be - one who was a 'match' to the target child in race but not language, and the other a 'mat… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

7
174
3
3

Year Published

2014
2014
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
3

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 141 publications
(187 citation statements)
references
References 41 publications
7
174
3
3
Order By: Relevance
“…Since racial essentialism develops with age (Kinzler & Dautel, 2012), it is no surprise that adults but not children endorse hypodescent when categorizing multiracial individuals (Roberts & Gelman, in press). Additional work is needed to uncover when in development racial essentialism emerges and interacts with racial biases to produce hypodescent.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Since racial essentialism develops with age (Kinzler & Dautel, 2012), it is no surprise that adults but not children endorse hypodescent when categorizing multiracial individuals (Roberts & Gelman, in press). Additional work is needed to uncover when in development racial essentialism emerges and interacts with racial biases to produce hypodescent.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, the present findings are in accordance with mounting evidence that gender is a more meaningful social distinction than race early in development (see Kinzler, Shutts, & Correll, 2010, and Shutts, 2013, for review). For example, infants, toddlers, and preschoolers are more likely to accept objects that are offered or endorsed by same- over other-gender individuals, but are equally likely to accept objects associated with same- and other-race individuals (Frazier et al, 2012; Kinzler & Spelke, 2011; Shutts, Banaji, & Spelke, 2010); young children treat gender, but not race, as a natural and stable category (Kinzler & Dautel, 2012; Rhodes & Gelman, 2009); and gender serves as a more robust guide to children’s friendship decisions in the preschool years than does race (Shutts, Roben, & Spelke, 2013). Further, when asked to infer an ad hoc categorization scheme (e.g., why an experimenter is putting stickers on some photographs but not others), children between three and eight years of age are more successful when categorization involves gender rather than race (McGraw, Durm, & Durnam, 1989; Mcgraw, Durm, & Patterson, 1983).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The images used in this study were drawn from the Radboud Faces Database (Langner et al, 2010), Child Affective Facial Expression Set (CAFE; Lobue & Thrasher, 2014), Kinzler and Dautel (2012), and online sources. The full CAFE set can be obtained at databrary.org.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%