2023
DOI: 10.1111/bjdp.12446
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Children's developing understanding of economic inequality and their place within it

Abstract: This document is made available in accordance with publisher policies and may differ from the published version or from the version of record. If you wish to cite this item you are advised to consult the publisher's version. Please see the URL above for details on accessing the published version.

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
17
0

Year Published

2023
2023
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 9 publications
(18 citation statements)
references
References 138 publications
1
17
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Breanne's emphasis on action and changing systemic conditions is consistent with a growing body of evidence that children Jenna's age can understand structural attributions (Dickinson et al, 2023) and that even young children are inclined to advocate for changing situations that they deem unfair (Elenbaas, 2019b).…”
Section: Structural Attributionsmentioning
confidence: 55%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Breanne's emphasis on action and changing systemic conditions is consistent with a growing body of evidence that children Jenna's age can understand structural attributions (Dickinson et al, 2023) and that even young children are inclined to advocate for changing situations that they deem unfair (Elenbaas, 2019b).…”
Section: Structural Attributionsmentioning
confidence: 55%
“…Amanda thinks that Lucy, "just needs to know that there are some families that just don't have GRIFFIN, MISTRY, YOUNG, AND MONTAGUE the resources." There is research evidence that young children of similar age to Lucy (i.e., ages 5 and 6) often do not make structural attributions for economic difference (Dickinson et al, 2023). However, in a year or two Lucy should be able to produce attributions for economic difference and we do not yet know if hearing attributions now might help the development of a diverse array of attributions for the causes of economic inequality.…”
Section: Nonclassifiable Attributionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Further, some participants believed their children were not aware of their family’s financial strain. While studies on this topic are scarce, existing literature reports the contrary; school-aged children in low-income households are aware of their family’s financial situation and have fears of being marginalized as a result [ 56 , 57 ], and there is evidence that children as young as 3 years have the cognitive capacity to recognize socioeconomic disadvantage and label themselves accordingly [ 58 ]. This suggests that an inconsistency exists between the lived experience of being a child in a low-income household and their parents’ perception of their experience.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Children are not shielded from economic contexts simply by virtue of their age; they encounter and encode structural and interpersonal cues of wealth and inequality in daily life (Diaz et al, 2023; Legaspi et al, 2023). There is evidence that by the age of 3, children can differentiate between rich and poor individuals based on appearance; by the age of 8, they possess a sophisticated enough understanding of economic stratification to be able to rank jobs by income; by the age of 14, they are aware of the structural forces at play within society, which enables them to connect inequality with economic and political systems (for reviews, see Dickinson et al, 2023; Elenbaas et al, 2020). Moreover, children are not blind to the types of motivations that energize and direct adult behavior.…”
Section: How Economic Inequality Fosters Competitive Motivations and ...mentioning
confidence: 99%