2023
DOI: 10.1037/dev0001455
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Children intend to teach conventional but not moral norms selectively to ingroup members.

Abstract: Several studies have investigated factors guiding children's decisions when learning from others, although less is known about factors that govern children's decisions when they transfer knowledge to others. Here we asked whether children would privilege ingroup members when teaching and, if so, whether this tendency would persist when transferring different kinds of information (conventional norms vs. moral norms). In Experiment 1 (N = 24), we first replicated ingroup preference based on minimal group members… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

0
7
0

Year Published

2023
2023
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 18 publications
(7 citation statements)
references
References 93 publications
0
7
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In light of this, people expect a cultural common ground between themselves and members of their group and adapt their social interactions and communication accordingly (Clark, 1996). Developmental studies suggest that, from an early age, children associate group membership and cultural knowledge and they guide their interactions with others based on this understanding (Diesendruck, 2005; Goldvicht-Bacon & Diesendruck, 2016; Karadağ & Soley, 2023; Liberman et al, 2018; Liebal et al, 2013). These studies mainly focused on children’s expectations about others’ epistemic states based on their social attributes, such as their familiarity or group membership.…”
Section: The Present Researchmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In light of this, people expect a cultural common ground between themselves and members of their group and adapt their social interactions and communication accordingly (Clark, 1996). Developmental studies suggest that, from an early age, children associate group membership and cultural knowledge and they guide their interactions with others based on this understanding (Diesendruck, 2005; Goldvicht-Bacon & Diesendruck, 2016; Karadağ & Soley, 2023; Liberman et al, 2018; Liebal et al, 2013). These studies mainly focused on children’s expectations about others’ epistemic states based on their social attributes, such as their familiarity or group membership.…”
Section: The Present Researchmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Emerging findings support the notion that children flexibly make selective decisions on what kind of information to transmit, to whom and when. They consider learners' goals and abilities (Gweon & Schulz, 2019), social group affiliation (Karadağ & Soley, 2022;, and occupations (Danovitch, 2020). Further, children do not transmit all learned information indiscriminately, but variably prioritize generalizable (Baer & Friedman, 2018;Gelman, Ware, Manczak, & Graham, 2013), cognitively opaque (Ronfard, Was, & Harris, 2016), simple (Bazhydai, Silverstein, Parise, & Westermann, 2020), and information acquired through explicit pedagogy .…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, instrumental stance-taking may explain the finding that children provided more comprehensive information to the learner who requested information to enable them to effectively complete an action, and also when the learner was introduced as being "silly" compared to exceptionally smart (Gweon & Schulz, 2019). Ritual stance-taking might account for children's use of normative language and enforcement of conventional norms selectively with ingroup but not outgroup members (Karadağ & Soley, 2022;. To directly investigate whether observed selectivity in information transmission stems from flexible stance-taking, future studies should focus on systematically manipulating factors that may potentially trigger these stances during transmissionthose regarding both the nature of information and the social attributes of the learner.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Emerging findings support the notion that children flexibly make selective decisions on what kind of information to transmit, to whom and when. They consider learners' goals and abilities (Gweon & Schulz, 2019), social group affiliation (Karadağ & Soley, 2022;Schmidt et al, 2012) and occupations (Danovitch, 2020). Further, children do not transmit all learned information indiscriminately, but variably prioritise generalizable (Baer & Friedman, 2018;Gelman et al, 2013), cognitively opaque (Ronfard et al, 2016), simple (Bazhydai, Silverstein et al, 2020), and information acquired through explicit pedagogy (Vredenburgh et al, 2015).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, instrumental stance-taking may explain the finding that children provided more comprehensive information to the learner who requested information to enable them to effectively complete an action, and also when the learner was introduced as being 'silly' compared to exceptionally smart (Gweon & Schulz, 2019). Ritual stance-taking might account for children's use of normative language and enforcement of conventional norms selectively with ingroup but not outgroup members (Karadağ & Soley, 2022;Schmidt et al, 2012). To directly investigate whether observed selectivity in information transmission stems from flexible stance-taking, future studies should focus on systematically manipulating factors that may potentially trigger these stances during transmission -those regarding both the nature of information and the social attributes of the learner.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%