1987
DOI: 10.1111/j.2044-835x.1987.tb01044.x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Children' conceptions of relations between messages, meanings and reality

Abstract: The paper is in six sections. In the first, we summarize interpretations of the results of laboratory investigations in which children give or receive information about a particular object or event in the real world. In these studies there is assumed to be a match between the speaker' intended meaning (an internal representation) and the intended referent in the outside world, and it appears that 5–6‐year‐old children are often oblivious to the importance of a one‐to‐one relationship between meaning/referent a… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

4
29
2

Year Published

1989
1989
2013
2013

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 37 publications
(35 citation statements)
references
References 15 publications
(19 reference statements)
4
29
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Normally developing children start to use context as soon as they start to understand language, but only (9) 9 (8.9) 0.34 9 (9) 0.00 9 (8.9) 0.34 Enrichment (9) 7.5 (7.2) 1.47 9 (8.7) 0.49 9 (8.5) 0.73 Routine (9) 8.0 (7.9) 1.10 8.0 (8.0) 1.11 9 (8.6) 0.72 Basic implicature (9) 7.5 (7.1) 1.31 8.0 (7.8) 1.10 9 (8.7) 0.49 Feeling ( after development progresses they learn to understand more sophisticated contextual clues and start to draw relevant inferences by combining contextual information from different sources (Bezuidenhout & Sroda, 1998;Jaswal & Markman, 2001;Lloyd, Camaioni, & Ercolani, 1995;Milosky, 1992;Robinson & Whittaker, 1987). In children with AS or HFA the pattern of performance on the different question types was approximately the same as is predicted by the relevance theory on the basis of contextual complexity (Leinonen et al, 2003;Sperber & Wilson, 1995, p. 179-182, 2004.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…Normally developing children start to use context as soon as they start to understand language, but only (9) 9 (8.9) 0.34 9 (9) 0.00 9 (8.9) 0.34 Enrichment (9) 7.5 (7.2) 1.47 9 (8.7) 0.49 9 (8.5) 0.73 Routine (9) 8.0 (7.9) 1.10 8.0 (8.0) 1.11 9 (8.6) 0.72 Basic implicature (9) 7.5 (7.1) 1.31 8.0 (7.8) 1.10 9 (8.7) 0.49 Feeling ( after development progresses they learn to understand more sophisticated contextual clues and start to draw relevant inferences by combining contextual information from different sources (Bezuidenhout & Sroda, 1998;Jaswal & Markman, 2001;Lloyd, Camaioni, & Ercolani, 1995;Milosky, 1992;Robinson & Whittaker, 1987). In children with AS or HFA the pattern of performance on the different question types was approximately the same as is predicted by the relevance theory on the basis of contextual complexity (Leinonen et al, 2003;Sperber & Wilson, 1995, p. 179-182, 2004.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…However, it has been argued on the basis of a different body of research that children younger than about 6 or 7 years of age fail to understand that messages are representations of speakers' intended meanings (Beal, 1988;Bonitatibus, 1988;Robinson, Goelman, & Olson, 1983;Robinson & Robinson, 1982;Robinson & Whittaker, 1987). One aim of this paper is to present new evidence which suggests that children's understanding of the relation between message and speakers' internal representation has been underestimated by these authors.…”
mentioning
confidence: 88%
“…As Robinson and Whittaker (1987) point out, we cannot assume that children who correctly identify ambiguous messages as such are treating messages as being clues to speakers' internal representations. This is because in the typical procedure there is a perfect match between the speaker's internal representation and the desired object in the outside world.…”
mentioning
confidence: 98%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…For example, under conditions of epistemic uncertainty as used in the published literature, Epistemic and physical uncertainty 11 children younger than around 7 years do not just make single interpretations of limited input but also evaluate the input as adequate (e.g. Beal & Flavell, 1982;Flavell, Speer, Green, & August, 1981;Robinson & Apperly, 2001;Robinson & Whittaker, 1987). They fail to explain why different people can make different interpretations of limited input (Carpendale & Chandler, 1996;Chandler, Hallet, & Sokol, 2002).…”
Section: Epistemic and Physical Uncertaintymentioning
confidence: 99%